Cerebral ventricular volume and temperamental difficulties in infancy. The Generation R Study ============================================================================================= * Sabine J. Roza * Paul P. Govaert * Maarten H. Lequin * Vincent W.V. Jaddoe * Henriette A. Moll * Eric A.P. Steegers * Albert Hofman * Frank C. Verhulst * Henning Tiemeier ## Abstract **Background:** Numerous studies have provided evidence for subtle deviations in brain morphology in children with psychiatric disorders, but much less is known about the onset and developmental trajectory of these deviations early in life. We sought to determine whether variances in cerebral ventricular size in fetuses and newborns are associated with temperamental difficulties in infants. **Methods:** Within a population-based cohort study, we measured the size of the lateral ventricle of the fetus’ brain twice during pregnancy. We used 3-dimensional cranial ultrasound to measure the cerebral ventricular volume of infants at age 6 weeks. We then related the size of the cerebral ventricular system to temperamental dimensions at age 3 months using the Mother and Baby Scales, and at age 6 months using the Infant Behavior Questionnaire for a total of 1028 infants. **Results:** The size of the lateral ventricle of the fetuses in midpregnancy was not related to temperamental difficulties in infants; however, smaller lateral ventricles in late pregnancy were associated with higher activity levels at the age of 6 months. Infants with smaller ventricular volumes at age 6 weeks experienced higher activity levels, more anger or irritability and poorer orienting later in infancy. Children with the lowest ventricular volumes scored on average 0.15 (95% confidence interval 0.06–0.23, *p* = 0.001) points higher (23%) on activity levels than children with the highest ventricular volumes. **Conclusion:** Variations in ventricular size before and shortly after birth are associated with temperamental difficulties. Some of the morphologic differences between children with and without psychiatric disorders may develop very early in life. ## Introduction In the last 2 decades, numerous studies have investigated the associations between anatomic brain structure and child and adolescent psychiatric disorders.1 Childhood psychiatric disorders, such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism and childhood-onset schizophrenia, are hypothesized to be neurodevelopmental in origin,2,3 although the underlying mechanisms that contribute to their development are yet unknown. Subtle changes in the brain may make individuals more vulnerable to affective disorders.1 In spite of the large number of studies that have used neuroimaging techniques to identify childhood psychiatric disorders, the findings thus far have been inconsistent,1,4,5 mainly owing to methodologic problems such as heterogeneity of disorders, small samples, different control groups and confounding because of pharmacotherapy. Nonetheless, the studies have shown correlations between measures of brain structure and neurodevelopmental disorders. For example, the most consistent structural abnormalities described in children with autism are enlarged total brain size6–8 and reduced corpus callosum size.9 Furthermore, abnormalities in the amygdala, hippocampus and cerebellum have been frequently reported in autistic children.7 It has been reported that the total brain size of children with ADHD is about 5% smaller than that of children of the same age and sex who do not have ADHD.10 Distributed subtle anomalies in the brains of children with ADHD have been described, in particular in the prefrontal cortices, right caudate, corpus callosum and cerebellar regions.11–13 Findings have been inconsistent with regard to the volume of the ventricular system, which is considered to be one of the general parameters of brain development.14 One study suggested that children with autism had enlarged lateral ventricles compared with children who did not have autism,15 whereas other studies did not report a statistical difference.16 Castellanos and colleagues10 described a slower increase in lateral ventricular volume in children with ADHD compared with children who did not have the disorder. Similarly, case series involving children with schizophrenia reported enlarged ventricles,4,17,18 a finding that is also consistent in adult patients with schizophrenia.19 However, it should be noted that lateral ventricular volume increases robustly with age in healthy children, which adds to the complexity of interpreting changes in ventricular volume in patient populations.20 It is unclear whether the subtle changes in the brains of children with psychiatric disorders emerge before the onset of symptoms or whether they are compensatory or adaptive changes of the nervous system. To elucidate whether and when the development of the brain is disturbed in children with psychiatric disorders, imaging techniques should be used to study progressively younger groups of children without disease at the time of imaging.21 Such studies have been conducted in groups of children (e.g., children born prematurely) at high risk of psychiatric disorders. Their results have shown that enlarged ventricles are very common in premature infants and are related to neurodevelopmental outcomes in the first years of life.22–25 Furthermore, mild ventricular enlargement in fetuses has been associated with ADHD, autism and learning disorders.26 However, studies involving patients at high risk for psychiatric disorders cannot distinguish brain damage due to prematurity, pharmacotherapy and mechanical ventilation from subtle changes in the developing brain due to genetic predisposition or environmental factors. One aspect of social and emotional behaviour that can be measured early in life is temperament. Temperament is conceptualized as the constitutionally based individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation, which are influenced over time by heredity, maturation and experience.27 Numerous studies have argued that temperament plays a role in the etiology and persistence of behavioural problems in childhood and adolescence.28–30 Precursors of behavioural problems include susceptibility to anger and hostility in infancy and early childhood.31 For example, ADHD symptom scores in kindergarten are predicted by irritability, anger, high activity levels and difficulties with inhibitory control in preschool-age children.32 Another study reported that children aged 7 years who had been classified as “high reactive” (i.e., vigorous motor activity combined with distress and crying) at the age of 4 months were more likely to have anxious symptoms than those who had been classified as “low reactive.”33 We sought to examine the association between ventricular size and temperamental difficulties in 1028 infants. We hypothesized that ventricular volumes of children with a more difficult temperament at age 3 months and 6 months would differ significantly from the ventricular volumes of children without temperamental problems. ## Methods ### Study population We conducted our study within a subgroup of the ongoing Generation R Study, which follows participants from fetal life until young adulthood in Rotterdam, the Netherlands. The Generation R Study previously has been described in detail.34,35 We selected fetuses and infants from the general population, and conducted detailed prenatal ultrasound assessments in 1232 pregnant women. This subgroup was racially homogeneous, which prevented confounding or effect modification based on ethnic origin. We did not define any other inclusion or exclusion criteria for participation in this subgroup. All children included in our study were born between February 2003 and August 2005. We measured ventricular size twice during pregnancy and once postnatally at the age of 6 weeks. We used 3-dimensional ultrasound as the imaging technique because, when compared with magnetic resonance imaging, ultrasound has been shown to provide valid volume measurements of the ventricular system.36 We performed fetal ultrasounds to measure cerebral ventricles during midpregnancy and late pregnancy. We then obtained data on temperament at ages 3 months and 6 months using postnatal questionnaires. We conducted our study in accordance with the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki, and we received approval from the Medical Ethics Committee of the Erasmus Medical Center in Rotterdam. We obtained written informed consent from all participants. ### Determinants Trained sonographers carried out fetal ultrasound examinations during mid- and late pregnancy. The median gestational age (and range) for these examinations were 21 (19–23) weeks for midpregnancy and 30 (28–33) weeks for late pregnancy. Online measurements included head circumference (the outer perimeter of the fetus’ skull) and the atrial width (the widest diameter of the atrium) of one of the lateral ventricles. Both were measured in an axial plane. High intraand interobserver reproducibility have been reported for both methods of measurement.37,38 We performed postnatal cranial ultrasounds with the Voluson 730 Expert (GE Healthcare), a commercially available multifrequency electronic transducer that could be used for 3-dimensional volume acquisition. We positioned the probe on the anterior fontanelle, and placed a volume box at the level of the interventricular foramen in a symmetric section of the crown. We scanned a pyramid-shaped volume of the brain tissue and measured the volume of the lateral ventricular system offline. This method was described elsewhere in detail.39 We quantified in millilitres the volume of the frontal horn of the lateral ventricle, ventricular body and ventricular trigone on both sides. Four raters manually traced the left and right ventricles. For reliability analyses, each rater segmented 20 images twice. All 4 then rated the images of another 20 children. The intra- and interobserver reliability of the volumetric measurements was very high, partly owing to the high variance in ventricular volume within our population. ### Infant temperament Mothers completed the Mother and Baby Scales (MABS)40,41 for their children at age 3 months and the Infant Behavior Questionnaire — Revised (IBQ-R)27 at age 6 months. We used the 2 infant scales of the MABS: infant irritability and alertness. The assessment scales ranged from 0 to 75 for irritability and from 0 to 40 for alertness. The irritability scale consisted of 15 items (e.g., “My baby has fussed before settling down”), and the alertness scale consisted of 8 items (e.g., “When I talk to my baby, (s)he seems to take notice”). We rated each item on a 6-point scale in which 0 indicated “not at all” and 5 indicated “very much/often.” A higher score on the irritability scale indicated more difficult behaviour, whereas a higher score on the alertness scale indicated less difficult behaviour. We chose 6 of 14 scales on the IBQ-R because use of the complete instrument, which includes 191 items, was not feasible within our multidisciplinary study with numerous assessments. The 6 scales we chose were activity level, distress to limitations, fear, duration of orienting, falling reactivity and sadness. We judged these to be of particular importance, from a clinical perspective, for the most prevalent behavioural disorders in childhood. Activity levels related to gross motor activity and squirming. Distress to limitations referred to negative emotionality and reaction to frustrating situations. Fear included rejection of new objects and persons. Duration of orienting comprised attention and distractibility. Falling reactivity referred to the rate of recovery from peak distress or frustration. Finally, questions about sadness related to lowered mood due to personal suffering or object loss. We rated each item on a 3-point scale in which 0 indicated “never present,” 1 indicated “sometimes present” and 2 indicated “often present.” Our scale was adapted from the original 7-point scale because participants of a pilot study seldom used the extreme positions on the scale and because reducing the number of options would not result in significant loss of information.42 Higher scores on all but falling reactivity indicated more difficult behaviour. We calculated the scores for each scale by dividing the sum of the items by the number of completed items. Because traits could be consolidated into a smaller number of dimensions,29 we grouped distress to limitations, recovery from distress and irritability into irritability or anger. We grouped fear and sadness into behavioural inhibition. We grouped duration of orienting and alertness into orienting.43 We did not group activity level into one of these categories because it represented the approach or positive affect dimension. ### Covariables We considered variables related to socioeconomic status, psychosocial stressors, and obstetric and neonatal variables as possible confounders. Furthermore, we adjusted our associations for other known determinants of infant temperament, including sex, age, maternal psychopathology and maternal self-esteem. We obtained information on the date of birth, birth weight and sex of the infant from community midwife and hospital registries at the time of birth. We established gestational age using the fetal ultrasound examinations within the Generation R Study. During the postnatal visit at age 6 weeks, we measured the fronto-occipital circumference of the head in centimetres. We determined maternal age, maternal educational level and parity at the time of enrolment. To assess maternal psychopathology during midpregnancy and at 3 months postpartum, we used the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI).44,45 We assessed family functioning using the 12-item General Functioning subscale of the McMaster Family Assessment Device.46 We measured longlasting difficulties using a 12-item checklist.47 We assessed maternal self-esteem in late pregnancy using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale.48 Community midwife and hospital registries at birth yielded information on mode of delivery and Apgar scores. We classified the mode of delivery as cesarean section, spontaneous vaginal delivery or instrumental vaginal delivery. ### Statistical analysis To examine whether nonresponse to the 3-month and 6-month questionnaires was selective, we compared core data on children for whom we obtained information on ventricular volume and temperament with data on eligible children for whom we were not able to obtain this information. We analyzed the associations between ventricular size and temperamental dimensions using multivariable linear regression models. First, we used log transformation of the data on postnatal ventricular volume to satisfy the assumption of normality. We used log or square root transformation of the data on the temperamental dimensions of fear, falling reactivity and alertness to achieve normality or constant variance. Next, we derived standard deviation scores for both determinants and outcomes to compare regression coefficients for transformed and nontransformed outcomes. Because the regression coefficients were not straightforward to interpret, we defined quartiles of ventricular volume and used analyses of covariance to compare the mean scores of nontransformed scales among the quartiles. We controlled all associations for head circumference when we measured the size of the ventricle because information about ventricular size was difficult to interpret without placing it in the context of overall head size. We selected covariables as a result of exploratory analyses, and included them in our analyses if effect estimates of ventricular size changed meaningfully (> 5%). We excluded colinear covariables to avoid instability of the models. For example, maternal self-esteem, family functioning and long-lasting difficulties were highly correlated to maternal psychopathology and had no additional confounding effect on the association between ventricular size and infant temperament. Other variables, such as maternal smoking, alcohol consumption, single parenthood, financial difficulties, obstetric or perinatal complications (e.g., Apgar score, mode of delivery, birth weight, gestational diabetes, maternal hypertension, pre-eclampsia), child anthropometrics and child birth order, did not confound the association between ventricular volume and infant temperament. Therefore, we did not include these variables in our final models. We performed a post-hoc Bonferroni adjustment for the multiple hypotheses regarding different temperamental scales per outcome in infants at ages 3 months and 6 months. When determinants and outcomes correlated highly, the conservatism of the Bonferroni adjustment increased, and the power of detecting effects owing to association was reduced. Because the ventricular measurements were highly correlated, we chose to correct for 2 hypotheses at age 3 months and for 6 hypotheses at age 6 months. We tested whether interaction terms of ventricular size and sex were significant at α = 0.05. Measures of association are presented with their 95% confidence intervals (CIs). We performed our analyses using the Statistical Package of Social Sciences version 11.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc.). ## Results ### Study population The 1232 mothers who participated in our study delivered 1244 babies (1 intrauterine fetal death, 2 neonatal deaths). We performed fetal ultrasounds to measure cerebral ventricles for 692 singleton fetuses during midpregnancy and for 1186 singleton fetuses during late pregnancy. We performed additional postnatal cranial ultrasounds of sufficient quality for 778 infants. Of the children for whom we obtained information on cerebral ventricles (*n* = 1233), data on temperament were available for 939 infants at the age of 3 months and for 769 infants at the age of 6 months. We were unable to obtain data on temperament because of withdrawn consent for postnatal questionnaires (*n* = 49), logistical problems in sending out the postnatal questionnaires (*n* = 113 for the 3-month questionnaire, *n* = 273 for the 6-month questionnaire) and nonresponse (*n* = 128 for the 3-month questionnaire, *n* = 138 for the 6-month questionnaire). Twenty mothers participated with 2 children each (twins or siblings). We randomly excluded 1 of the siblings to avoid biases due to paired data. We included a total of 1028 children (82.6% of 1244) in one or more of our analyses. Table 1 presents the characteristics of participating children and their parents. Half (50.2%) of the infants were boys. Birth weight and the circumference of the head at the gestational ages of 20 and 30 weeks, and at the postnatal age of 6 weeks, were larger among boys than girls. The median gestational duration was 40.1 weeks (range 28–43 wk). In total, 40 of 1028 children (3.9%) were born before 37 weeks. The mean maternal age at the time of enrolment was 31.8 years (range 18–46 yr). Of all participating children, 637 (62%) were the first-born children in their families. The mean measurements of atrial width of the lateral ventricle (and standard deviations [SD]) were 5.8 mm (1.2 mm) in midpregnancy, 5.3 mm (1.7 mm) in late pregnancy and −0.26 mm (0.7 mm) for the log transformation of data on postnatal ventricular volume. We found that the volume of the left ventricle was on average higher (median 0.44 mL, range 0.1 mL–1.6 mL) than that of the right ventricle (median 0.36 mL, range 0.1 mL–1.3 mL). Intraobserver intraclass correlation coefficients varied from 0.989 to 0.993 for the right ventricle and from 0.992 to 0.997 for the left ventricle. Interobserver intraclass correlation coefficients were 0.950 for the right ventricle and 0.981 for the left ventricle. View this table: [Table 1](http://jpn.ca/content/33/5/431/T1) Table 1 Characteristics of participants included in the analysis of association between cerebral ventricular volume and temperamental difficulties in infancy Table 2 presents the associations between ventricular size and temperament at the age of 3 months. Ventricular size in mid- and late pregnancy was not related to temperament in 3-month-old infants; however, smaller ventricular volumes measured postnatally were associated with orienting at the age of 3 months. For each unit of decreased standard deviation related to lower ventricular volume, the infant alertness score also decreased by 0.10. This association remained statistically significant after we performed a Bonferroni adjustment. We found no significant differences between the sexes. Head circumference in late pregnancy was related to infant alertness only (β −0.01, 95% CI −0.01 to −0.00, *p* = 0.006). However, the associations between ventricular size and infant temperament could not be explained by head circumference. View this table: [Table 2](http://jpn.ca/content/33/5/431/T2) Table 2 Associations between ventricular size and temperament at age 3 months observed in 912 infants* We related ventricular size in midpregnancy, late pregnancy and at the age of 6 weeks to infant temperament at the age of 6 months (Table 3). We found that ventricular size in midpregnancy was not associated with infant temperament. In late pregnancy, smaller ventricular size predicted only higher activity levels at the age of 6 months. Postnatally, smaller ventricular volume was associated with both anger or irritability and with higher activity levels. For each unit of decreased standard deviation related to smaller ventricular volume at age 6 weeks, the activity level score at age 6 months increased by 0.18, and the infant distress to limitations score increased by 0.14. The rate of recovery from distress score decreased by 0.12. We found no significant associations between postnatal ventricular volumes with fear, sadness or duration of orienting. After performing the Bonferroni adjustment to establish more conservative alphas (α = 0.0083), we found that the associations between postnatal ventricular volume and activity levels and distress to limitations remained statistically significant. None of the interaction terms of ventricular size and sex was significant at α = 0.15. The circumference of the head at the age of 6 weeks was associated with distress to limitations (β = 0.06, 95% CI 0.00–0.12, *p* = 0.04); however, the measurement did not account for the association between ventricular size and infant temperament. Moreover, exclusion of children born prematurely (gestational age at birth < 37 wk, *n* = 40) did not change our results. View this table: [Table 3](http://jpn.ca/content/33/5/431/T3) Table 3 Associations between ventricular size and temperament at age 6 months in 745 infants* Internal consistencies for scores on the irritability and alertness scales were α = 0.75 for irritability and α = 0.64 for alertness. Figure 1 presents the adjusted mean scores on activity level, distress to limitations and sadness, per quartile of ventricular volume. Children with lower ventricular volumes scored on average 0.15 (95% CI 0.06–0.23, *p* = 0.001) points higher (23%) on activity levels compared with children with the highest ventricular volumes. The mean score on distress to limitations for children with the lowest ventricular volumes was 0.09 (95% CI 0.01–0.17, *p* = 0.03) points higher (17%) than the mean score of those with the highest ventricular volumes. The difference in scores on sadness between children in these quartiles was not statistically significant (0.05 points, 95% CI −0.03 to 0.12, *p* = 0.21). ![Fig. 1](http://jpn.ca/https://www.jpn.ca/content/jpn/33/5/431/F1.medium.gif) [Fig. 1](http://jpn.ca/content/33/5/431/F1) Fig. 1 Mean activity level, distress to limitations and sadness scores (on scales ranging from 0 to 2) associated with postnatal ventricular volume in 1028 infants. Values are estimated means and 95% confidence intervals, adjusted for age, sex, gestational duration, head circumference at the time of a 3-dimensional postnatal ultrasound, maternal age, maternal education level and maternal psychopathology. Analyses of missing data showed that the birth weight of children for whom data on determinants or outcomes was not available was, on average, 119 g (95% CI 16 g–22 g) lower than the birth weights of children for whom data were available. The mothers of children for whom data were missing were 1.3 years (95% CI 0.5–2.1 yr) younger than those of children for whom data were available. In addition, 51.4% of the mothers of children for whom data were missing had completed some higher education compared with 64.9% of the mothers of children for whom data were available (χ22 = 22.3, *p* < 0.001). We found no differences between the 2 groups of children regarding median gestational duration or parental psychopathology. We found that ventricular sizes measured in midpregnancy, late pregnancy and postnatally at the age of 6 weeks did not differ between children for whom data on temperament were missing and those for whom data were available. ## Discussion We found an association between variation in brain morphology in fetuses and newborns and temperamental difficulties at ages 3 months and 6 months. Infants with a smaller cerebral ventricular system were more likely to have temperamental problems. Ventricular volume in infants was related to alertness at the age of 3 months. Furthermore, lower ventricular volume in infants was associated with more anger or irritability and higher activity levels at age 6 months. The size of the prenatal cerebral ventricular system was less strongly associated with infant temperament. Smaller ventricles in late pregnancy were related only to higher activity levels at age 6 months, and ventricular size in midpregnancy did not predict temperamental problems. To our knowledge, our study was the first to attempt to relate brain morphology to behaviour within the normal population at such a young age. Early behavioural and emotional characteristics, or temperamental features, of very young children are thought to have a constitutional basis and form a relatively enduring makeup of the individual.49 Temperamental traits have been frequently related to behaviour and psychopathology in childhood and later in life. Higher infant activity levels are related to outgoing activity and positive anticipation as well as impulsivity, anger or frustration and lower inhibitory control. Irritability and frustration in infancy predicted later externalizing negative affect43 as well as oppositional and conduct problems.29 Moreover, hostile and aggressive behaviour, anger and difficulties with inhibitory control are precursors of ADHD.32 This suggests that the smaller ventricles observed among fetuses and infants in our study indicate a higher risk for developing externalizing psychopathology, including ADHD. Most reports on ventricular volume and childhood psychiatric disorders described larger ventricles in patients with childhood-onset schizophrenia, autism, depression and eating disorders.4 However, interpreting changes in ventricular volume in patient populations is complex, since studies involving children and adolescents with normal development reported robust increases of lateral ventricular volume with age.20 The interpretation of this complexity would benefit from prospective studies in populations that are epidemiologically ascertained, which might help to identify the longitudinal trajectories of abnormalities in the brain that are related to illness.21 Our results indicated that lower ventricular volume in infancy may increase the risk of behavioural problems. This is consistent with the findings of Castellanos and colleagues,10 who described a diminished age-related increase in lateral ventricular volume from ages 5–18 years in children with ADHD compared with children who did not have ADHD. Because knowledge on the maturation of the ventricular system is limited, we can only speculate about the underlying mechanism of the association between lower ventricular volumes and temperamental difficulties. It is possible that lower ventricular volumes are related to delayed or reduced apoptosis of overproduced neurons, which is one of the regressive phenomena that can occur during the development of the nervous system.50,51 Another possible mechanism that explains the association between small ventricles and temperamental difficulties might be the reduced secretion of cerebrospinal fluid, which leads to a loss of the essential expanding pressure within the ventricular system that might determine brain morphology during its development.52 Small changes in brain development during prenatal and early postnatal life may be the consequence of several adverse environmental factors in utero. First, placental insufficiency may lead to chronic fetal hypoxia and to a shortage of other nutrients such as glucose and amino acids.53 This not only leads to intrauterine growth restriction, but may also lead to altered brain structure and abnormalities in brain function.54 Second, maternal smoking during pregnancy and maternal gestational stress have been shown to have direct negative effects on the developing brain.55,56 These environmental factors have also been related to behavioural problems later in life. ### Limitations Although the effect sizes were small, the association between a lower ventricular volume and temperamental problems were consistent among children aged 3 months and 6 months. This association was pronounced for problems related to externalizing behaviour. However, several other methodologic considerations must be discussed. First, selective participation of healthier, more highly educated and older mothers may have impeded the detection of modest associations. We had no indication that the size of the lateral ventricle was different in children of younger and less educated women. The selective attrition of mothers with prenatally and perinatally complicated pregnancies could have led to bias. Their children may have had larger lateral ventricles postnatally owing to white matter damage or intraventricular hemorrhage and, in consequence, more behavioural problems. Therefore, we should be cautious in generalizing our findings to clinical populations such as children born prematurely. Second, the lower resolution of ultrasound compared with magnetic resonance imaging necessitated manual tracing of ventricles and precluded tracing of other regions of the brain. However, our methods of ventricular measurement were highly reproducible, as indicated by high intra- and interobserver reliability scores. Third, our use of questionnaires for mothers to report their children’s temperamental traits may have introduced reporter bias.57 However, we assumed this type of misclassification was nondifferential, since mothers were blinded to their infants’ brain morphologies. Furthermore, we adjusted our associations for maternal psychopathology, which did not fully explain the associations between ventricular size and infant temperament. Although Seifer and colleagues57 suggested that laboratory or home observations reliably and validly measured infant temperament and corresponded poorly with parent reports, maternal reporting of temperament took advantage of the primary caregiver’s opportunity to observe her infant in different contexts.27 Fourth, the limited categories in our adapted version of the IBQ-R may have reduced the variation of temperamental scores. This would decrease our chance of detecting statistically significant effects. Fifth, our study could not provide direct information on the developing trajectory of lateral ventricles throughout pregnancy and infancy because the 2-dimensional measurements of the atrium during pregnancy and the 3-dimensional measurement of the frontal horns and ventricular body after birth were not equivalent. We previously reported that a smaller size and a decreasing growth pattern of the width of the atrium in the lateral ventricle during pregnancy were predictive of lower ventricular volume in infancy.39 However, our results suggest that postnatal measurement of ventricular size is superior to prenatal ventricle measurement in predicting infant temperament. Finally, owing to the short time period between measurement of postnatal ventricular volume and assessment of temperamental outcome, inferences about cause and effect have to be made with caution. ### Implications Our study provides direct evidence that cerebral ventricular volume in infancy is related to temperamental difficulties. This suggests that the differences in brain morphology between children with and without specific psychiatric disorders could develop very early in life. Although it may be premature to speculate about clinical implications such as cranial ultrasound screening, our results suggested that it is worthwhile to follow up chance findings of very low ventricular volume in term infants, particularly when accompanied by maternal concerns about her child’s temperamental difficulties. Future studies should address the development of brain structure from infancy onward in relation to emotional and behavioural problems in childhood and adolescence. Ideally, this should be investigated in large prospective studies involving the general population or patients at high risk for psychiatric disorders. ## Acknowledgements The Generation R Study is conducted by the Erasmus Medical Center Rotterdam in close collaboration with the Faculty of Social Sciences of the Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Municipal Health Service Rotterdam area, the Rotterdam Homecare Foundation and the Stichting Trombosedienst & Artsenlaboratorium Rijnmond (STAR), Rotterdam, the Netherlands. We gratefully acknowledge the contribution of general practitioners, hospitals, midwives and pharmacies in Rotterdam. The first phase of the Generation R Study is made possible by financial support from the Erasmus Medical Center Rotterdam, the Erasmus University Rotterdam and the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw). This study was supported by an additional grant from the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw, ‘Geestkracht’ programme 10.000.1003). ## Footnotes * Medical subject headings: attention deficit and disruptive behaviour disorders; fetal development; child development. * **Competing interests:** None declared. * **Contributors:** Drs. Roza, Govaert, Verhulst and Tiemeier designed the study. All authors acquired the data. Drs. Roza and Tiemeier analyzed the data and wrote the article. Drs. Govaert, Lequin, Jaddoe, Moll, Steegers, Hofman and Verhulst reviewed the article. All authors gave final approval for publication. * Received August 31, 2007. * Revision received November 30, 2007. * Revision received January 19, 2008. * Accepted January 21, 2008. ## References 1. 1. Cicchetti D, 2. Cohen D Giedd JN, Shaw P, Wallace G, et al. Anatomic brain imaging studies of normal and abnormal brain development in children and adolescents. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental psychopathology. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley; 2006. p. 127–96. 2. Filipek PA. Neuroimaging in the developmental disorders: the state of the science. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999;40:113–28. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0021963098003308&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10102728&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000078874100006&link_type=ISI) 3. Bradshaw JL, Sheppard DM. The neurodevelopmental frontostriatal disorders: evolutionary adaptiveness and anomalous lateralization. Brain Lang 2000;73:297–320. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1006/brln.2000.2308&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10856179&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000087669800008&link_type=ISI) 4. Hendren RL, De Backer I, Pandina GJ. Review of neuroimaging studies of child and adolescent psychiatric disorders from the past 10 years. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000;39:815–28. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/00004583-200007000-00010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10892223&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000087817100010&link_type=ISI) 5. Eliez S, Reiss AL. MRI neuroimaging of childhood psychiatric disorders: a selective review. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;41:679–94. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0021963099006095&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11039681&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000089630300002&link_type=ISI) 6. Redcay E, Courchesne E. When is the brain enlarged in autism? A meta-analysis of all brain size reports. Biol Psychiatry 2005;58:1–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.026&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15935993&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000230260100001&link_type=ISI) 7. DiCicco-Bloom E, Lord C, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. The developmental neurobiology of autism spectrum disorder. J Neurosci 2006;26:6897–906. [FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiRlVMTCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjEwOiIyNi8yNi82ODk3IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTg6Ii9qcG4vMzMvNS80MzEuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 8. Courchesne E, Redcay E, Morgan JT, et al. Autism at the beginning: microstructural and growth abnormalities underlying the cognitive and behavioral phenotype of autism. Dev Psychopathol 2005;17:577–97. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0954579405050285&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16262983&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000233772000002&link_type=ISI) 9. Waiter GD, Williams JH, Murray AD, et al. Structural white matter deficits in high-functioning individuals with autistic spectrum disorder: a voxel-based investigation. Neuroimage 2005;24:455–61. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.049&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15627587&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000226454500018&link_type=ISI) 10. Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Marsh WL, et al. Quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996;53:607–16. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830070053009&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8660127&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1996UW18200006&link_type=ISI) 11. Krain AL, Castellanos FX. Brain development and ADHD. Clin Psychol Rev 2006;26:433–44. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.005&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16480802&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000239481300004&link_type=ISI) 12. Valera EM, Faraone SV, Murray KE, et al. Meta-analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2007;61:1361–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.011&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16950217&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000247127100006&link_type=ISI) 13. Seidman LJ, Valera EM, Makris N. Structural brain imaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57: 1263–72. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.019&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15949998&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000229570500009&link_type=ISI) 14. Gilmore JH, van Tol J, Kliewer MA, et al. Mild ventriculomegaly detected in utero with ultrasound: clinical associations and implications for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1998;33:133–40. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00073-5&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9789905&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000076198000002&link_type=ISI) 15. Piven J, Arndt S, Bailey J, et al. An MRI study of brain size in autism. Am J Psychiatry 1995;152:1145–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/ajp.152.8.1145&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7625461&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1995RL64200007&link_type=ISI) 16. Hardan AY, Minshew NJ, Mallikarjuhn M, et al. Brain volume in autism. J Child Neurol 2001;16:421–4. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/088307380101600607&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11417608&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000171025800007&link_type=ISI) 17. Woody RC, Bolyard K, Eisenhauer G, et al. CT scan and MRI findings in a child with schizophrenia. J Child Neurol 1987;2:105–10. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/088307388700200205&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=3598138&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1987G905400005&link_type=ISI) 18. Schulz SC, Koller MM, Kishore PR, et al. Ventricular enlargement in teenage patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1983;140:1592–5. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=6650689&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1983RU09000010&link_type=ISI) 19. Wright IC, Rabe-Hesketh S, Woodruff PW, et al. Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157: 16–25. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/ajp.157.1.16&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10618008&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000084559900004&link_type=ISI) 20. Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30:718–29. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.001&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16887188&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000241208800002&link_type=ISI) 21. Peterson BS. Conceptual, methodological, and statistical challenges in brain imaging studies of developmentally based psychopathologies. Dev Psychopathol 2003;15:811–32. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0954579403000385&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14582941&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000185309300013&link_type=ISI) 22. Thompson DK, Warfield SK, Carlin JB, et al. Perinatal risk factors altering regional brain structure in the preterm infant. Brain 2007;130:667–77. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/brain/awl277&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17008333&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000244840000008&link_type=ISI) 23. Inder TE, Warfield SK, Wang H, et al. Abnormal cerebral structure is present at term in premature infants. Pediatrics 2005;115:286–94. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6InBlZGlhdHJpY3MiO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6OToiMTE1LzIvMjg2IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTg6Ii9qcG4vMzMvNS80MzEuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 24. Miller SP, Ferriero DM, Leonard C, et al. Early brain injury in premature newborns detected with magnetic resonance imaging is associated with adverse early neurodevelopmental outcome. J Pediatr 2005;147:609–16. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jpeds.2005.06.033&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16291350&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000233500000013&link_type=ISI) 25. Dyet LE, Kennea N, Counsell SJ, et al. Natural history of brain lesions in extremely preterm infants studied with serial magnetic resonance imaging from birth and neurodevelopmental assessment. Pediatrics 2006;118:536–48. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6InBlZGlhdHJpY3MiO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6OToiMTE4LzIvNTM2IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTg6Ii9qcG4vMzMvNS80MzEuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 26. Gilmore JH, van Tol JJ, Lewis Streicher H, et al. Outcome in children with fetal mild ventriculomegaly: a case series. Schizophr Res 2001;48:219–26. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00140-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11295375&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000168220300007&link_type=ISI) 27. Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev 2003;26:64–86. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00169-8&link_type=DOI) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000180986900005&link_type=ISI) 28. 1. Cicchetti D, 2. Cohen D Rothbart MK, Posner MI. Temperament, attention, and developmental psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental psychopathology. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley; 2006. p. 465–501. 29. Nigg JT. Temperament and developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006;47:395–422. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16492265&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000235498500009&link_type=ISI) 30. Muris P, Ollendick TH. The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2005;8:271–89. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s10567-005-8809-y&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16362256&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) 31. Frick PJ, Morris AS. Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2004;33:54–68. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_6&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15028541&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000188884400006&link_type=ISI) 32. Nigg JT, Goldsmith HH, Sachek J. Temperament and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the development of a multiple pathway model. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2004;33:42–53. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_5&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15028540&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000188884400005&link_type=ISI) 33. Kagan J, Snidman N, Zentner M, et al. Infant temperament and anxious symptoms in school age children. Dev Psychopathol 1999; 11:209–24. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0954579499002023&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16506531&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000081287600002&link_type=ISI) 34. Hofman A, Jaddoe VW, Mackenbach JP, et al. Growth, development and health from early fetal life until young adulthood: the Generation R Study. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2004;18:61–72. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1365-3016.2003.00521.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14738548&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000188354200009&link_type=ISI) 35. Jaddoe VW, Mackenbach JP, Moll HA, et al. The Generation R Study: design and cohort profile. Eur J Epidemiol 2006;21:475–84. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s10654-006-9022-0&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16826450&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000239890600010&link_type=ISI) 36. Gilmore JH, Gerig G, Specter B, et al. Infant cerebral ventricle volume: a comparison of 3-D ultrasound and magnetic resonance imaging. Ultrasound Med Biol 2001;27:1143–6. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0301-5629(01)00400-8&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11527602&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) 37. Perni SC, Chervenak FA, Kalish RB, et al. Intraobserver and interobserver reproducibility of fetal biometry. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol 2004;24:654–8. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15476300&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) 38. Cardoza JD, Goldstein RB, Filly RA. Exclusion of fetal ventriculomegaly with a single measurement: the width of the lateral ventricular atrium. Radiology 1988;169:711–4. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1148/radiology.169.3.3055034&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=3055034&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1988Q972700029&link_type=ISI) 39. Roza SJ, Govaert PP, Vrooman HA, et al. Foetal growth determines cerebral ventricular volume in infants. The Generation R Study. Neuroimage 2008;39:1491–8. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18089296&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) 40. St James-Roberts I, Wolke D. Convergences and discrepancies, among mothers’ and professionals’ assessments of difficult neonatal behaviour. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1988;29:21–42. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1469-7610.1988.tb00686.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=3350881&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1988M107100003&link_type=ISI) 41. 1. Rauh H, 2. Steinhausen HC Wolke D, St James-Roberts I. Multi-method measurement of the early parent–infant system with easy and difficult newborns. In: Rauh H, Steinhausen HC, editors. Psychobiology and early development. North-Holland: Oxford; 1987. p. 49–70. 42. Streiner DL, Norman GR. Health Measurement Scales: a practical guide to their development and use. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. 43. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE. Temperament and personality: origins and outcomes. J Pers Soc Psychol 2000;78:122–35. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10653510&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000084760900010&link_type=ISI) 44. de Beurs E. Brief Symptom Inventory, handleiding. Leiden, the Netherlands: Psychologische Instrumenten Tests en Services; 2004. 45. Derogatis LR, Melisaratos N. The Brief Symptom Inventory: an introductory report. Psychol Med 1983;13:595–605. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0033291700048017&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=6622612&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1983RG21300014&link_type=ISI) 46. Byles J, Byrne C, Boyle MH, et al. Ontario Child Health Study: reliability and validity of the general functioning subscale of the McMaster Family Assessment Device. Fam Process 1988;27:97–104. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1545-5300.1988.00097.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=3360100&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1988M319600010&link_type=ISI) 47. Hendriks AAJ, Ormel J, van de Willige G. Long lasting difficulties measured with a self assessment questionnaire and semi structured interview: a theoretical and empirical comparison [in Dutch]. Gedrag Gezond 1990;18:273–83. 48. Rosenberg M. Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books; 1979. 49. Whittle S, Allen NB, Lubman DI, et al. The neurobiological basis of temperament: towards a better understanding of psychopathology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30:511–25. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.09.003&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16289282&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000238464500005&link_type=ISI) 50. Chugani HT. Biological basis of emotions: brain systems and brain development. Pediatrics 1998;102(Suppl E):1225–9. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6InBlZGlhdHJpY3MiO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6MjI6IjEwMi9TdXBwbGVtZW50X0UxLzEyMjUiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoxODoiL2pwbi8zMy81LzQzMS5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 51. Webb SJ, Monk CS, Nelson CA. Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological development: implications for human development. Dev Neuropsychol 2001;19:147–71. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1207/S15326942DN1902_2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11530973&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000170617200002&link_type=ISI) 52. Dziegielewska KM, Ek J, Habgood MD, et al. Development of the choroid plexus. Microsc Res Tech 2001;52:5–20. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/1097-0029(20010101)52:1<5::AID-JEMT3>3.0.CO;2-J&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11135444&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000166132200003&link_type=ISI) 53. Baschat AA. Fetal responses to placental insufficiency: an update. BJOG 2004;111:1031–41. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1471-0528.2004.00273.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15383103&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000223995700002&link_type=ISI) 54. Rehn AE, Van Den Buuse M, Copolov D, et al. An animal model of chronic placental insufficiency: relevance to neurodevelopmental disorders including schizophrenia. Neuroscience 2004;129:381–91. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.07.047&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15501595&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000224973200011&link_type=ISI) 55. Ernst M, Moolchan ET, Robinson ML. Behavioral and neural consequences of prenatal exposure to nicotine. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001;40:630–41. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/00004583-200106000-00007&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11392340&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000168786400007&link_type=ISI) 56. Davis EP, Glynn LM, Schetter CD, et al. Prenatal exposure to maternal depression and cortisol influences infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2007;46:737–46. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/chi.0b013e318047b775&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17513986&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000246733900008&link_type=ISI) 57. Seifer R, Sameroff AJ, Barrett LC, et al. Infant temperament measured by multiple observations and mother report. Child Dev 1994;65:1478–90. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.2307/1131512&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7982363&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F33%2F5%2F431.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1994PR01700016&link_type=ISI)