Does neuroimaging research examining the pathophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder require medication-free patients? =========================================================================================================================== * Ruth A. Lanius * Chris R. Brewin * J. Douglas Bremner * Judith K. Daniels * Matthew J. Friedman * Israel Liberzon * Alexander McFarlane * Paula P. Schnurr * Lisa Shin * Murray Stein * Eric Vermetten ## Abstract **Background:** In an attempt to avoid unknown influence, most neuroimaging studies examining the pathophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) exclude patients taking medications. Here we review the empirical evidence for relevant medications having a confounding effect on task performance or cerebral blood flow (CBF) in this population. The evidence for potentially confounding effects of psychotherapy in PTSD are also discussed. **Methods:** The literature that we reviewed was obtained through a PubMed search from 1980 to 2009 using the search terms posttraumatic stress disorder, PTSD, psychotropic medications, neuroimaging, functional magnetic resonance imaging, positron emission tomography, cerebral blood flow, CBF, serotonin-specific reuptake blocker, benzodiazepine, ketamine, methamphetamine, lamotrigine and atypical antipsychotic agents. **Results:** The empirical evidence for relevant medications having a confounding effect on task performance or CBF in relevant areas remains sparse for most psychotropic medications among patients with PTSD. However, considerable evidence is accumulating for 2 of the most commonly prescribed medication classes (serotonin-specific reuptake inhibitors and benzodiazepines) in healthy controls. Compelling data for the potentially confounding effects on brain areas relevant to PTSD for psychotherapeutic interventions are also accumulating. **Conclusion:** Neuroimaging studies examining the pathophysiology of PTSD should ideally recruit both medicated (assuming that the medication treatment has not resulted in the remission of symptoms) and unmedicated participants, to allow the findings to be generalized with greater confidence to the entire population of patients with PTSD. More research is needed into the independent effects of medications on task performance and CBF in regions of interest in PTSD. Neuro-imaging studies should also take into account whether patients are currently engaged in psychotherapeutic treatment. ## Introduction In the last decade, functional neuroimaging research in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) has resulted in an explosion of new data implicating the brain circuits involved in the patho-physiology of this disorder. Studies of cerebral blood flow (CBF) using positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) have examined neural responses to script-driven imagery, to viewing fearful, happy and neutral faces, and to non–trauma-related scripts or pictures.1 A recent meta-analysis has shown that PTSD patients tend to exhibit greater brain activation than healthy controls in the amygdala and insula, structures involved in fear conditioning and in the perception of bodily states, respectively. Moreover, decreased activation of the dorsal and rostral anterior cingulate and altered activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, regions involved in the experience and regulation of emotion, have repeatedly been observed in patients with PTSD.1 In an attempt to avoid unknown influences, most neuro-imaging studies examining the pathophysiology of PTSD thus far have included predominantly medication-free patients; only a few studies have included patients taking psychotropic medications.2–4 In this article, we argue that the assumption that medication use will affect the results carries with it a number of important costs that are not generally acknowledged. Moreover, in what appears to be an example of dualistic reasoning, there has been less focus on excluding patients receiving psychologic interventions for PTSD, even though these interventions may also affect neuroimaging findings. We conclude with recommendations for future investigations with regard to the use of psychotropic medications and psychotherapy in neuroimaging studies. The primary rationale for including only medication-free patients is to eliminate the potentially confounding factor of medication use. This confounding factor may exist in 3 main forms: medication may influence task performance independent of symptom levels; medication may independently influence blood flow or blood oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) effects in areas implicated in the condition under study; and medication may independently influence neural activity that reflects the interaction between the study group and task. To address the assumption that it is best to include only medication-free patients, we review the evidence for the first 2 types of confounding effects in PTSD by examining studies that have compared outcomes in medicated and unmedicated participants. However, it should be noted that because medication status tends not to be experimentally controlled, these comparisons may be influenced by factors correlated with medication status that could conspire to create and/or eliminate true differences. To date, no studies have examined the influence of specific medications on task performance in PTSD using neuroimaging methods. ## Methods We obtained the literature reviewed in this article through a PubMed search dating between 1980 and 2009 using the search terms posttraumatic stress disorder, PTSD, psycho-tropic medications, neuroimaging, functional magnetic resonance imaging, positron emission tomography, cerebral blood flow, CBF, serotonin-specific reuptake blockers, benzo-diazepines, ketamine, methamphetamine, lamotrigine and atypical antipsychotic agents. In addition, we also included articles identified from the reference lists of the articles obtained in this search. ## Results ### Effects of psychotropic medications on task performance in PTSD Various nonneuroimaging studies involving PTSD patients have examined medication effects in a variety of different paradigms. Two studies from the same research group examined physiologic responses to loud tones in veteran PTSD patients5 and monozygotic Vietnam veteran twins discordant for combat exposure.6 Whereas the specific medications used in the veteran sample were not reported, the studies included as a covariate in the analysis the use of one or more potentially confounding medications, including narcotics, anti -depressants, neuroleptics and benzodiazepines. Both studies found that the inclusion of patients taking prescription medications did not alter the findings of increased autonomic responses to loud tone stimuli in patients with PTSD. In the same sample of twins, the presence of reduced recall for fear extinction was also examined. Retention of extinction of conditioned fear was deficient in the PTSD group. The PTSD combat veterans showed larger skin conductance responses during extinction recall compared with their own twins as well as compared with non-PTSD combat veterans and their twins.6 These results were again not affected by the use of psychotropic medications, as determined by an analysis of covariance for medication status.7 A study of reward function in patients with combat-related PTSD used pictures of beautiful male and female faces and found no difference in responses between the medicated and unmedicated patients; both medicated and unmedicated combat-related PTSD veterans groups expended less effort by pressing a particular button to extend the viewing time of these pictures compared with Vietnam veterans without PTSD.8 The specific medications used in the study were not reported. A study investigating neurocognitive function, including visual memory, attention and encoding, executive function and visuospatial ability in medicated and unmedicated twins discordant for combat exposure showed that the results were not affected by current psychotropic medication use.9 Whereas no analysis by medication type was reported, the authors specify that 50% of medicated veterans reported benzodiazepine use, 25% reported use of a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), 42% were prescribed a serotonin modulator and 25% reported taking multiple medications, which included a benzodiazepine in addition to a SSRI or a serotonin modulator in all cases. A study of attention, learning and memory performance and estimated intellectual potential in Vietnam veterans also failed to find significant differences between medicated and unmedicated PTSD subgroups. Both groups of patients exhibited cognitive deficits on tasks of sustained attention, working memory, initial learning and estimated premorbid intelligence, but not on measures of focus of attention, shift of attention or memory savings.10 In addition, neither Golier and colleagues11 nor Nixon and colleagues12 reported a relation between memory performance and the use of psychotropic medications. The latter 2 studies did not report the specific psychotropic medications used by the participants. Orr and colleagues13 examined psychophysiologic responses in a group of women with PTSD due to childhood sexual abuse by use of personal sexual abuse imagery (a method similar to script-driven imagery symptom provocation neuroimaging studies). The investigators found that the larger physiologic responses of heart rate and skin conductance in the PTSD group were not significantly different in medicated versus unmedicated subgroups; however, the specific medications used in these studies were not reported. Similar results were found in studies examining olfactory identification as a measure of orbitofrontal integrity and qualitative block design performance in Vietnam veterans with PTSD.14,15 Although it is noteworthy that neither one of these studies could establish a significant correlation between medication status and task performance, interpretation of these findings is limited owing to a lack of medication-specific analyses. In contrast, a study that examined auditory event-related potential, especially the parietal P3 amplitude, to tone stimuli in combat-related PTSD reported abnormalities in unmedicated patients (*n* = 8) that were not present in a group of patients medicated (*n* = 16) with either an SSRI (*n* = 11), tra-zodone (*n* = 6), lithium (*n* = 1), carbamazepine (*n* = 1), buspirone (*n* = 2), neuroleptics (*n* = 4) or benzodiazepines (*n* = 1). Medicated patients did not differ significantly from those in the control group without PTSD, and the authors suggested that psychotropic medications may normalize evoked potential-related deficits in PTSD.16 However, to substantiate this hypothesis further, carefully designed studies that analyze the specific effect of medication groups on psychophysiologic parameters are warranted. In summary, the majority of studies that involved subgroups taking or not taking psychotropic medication and studied psychophysical responses, neurocognitive functioning and reward functioning have not found a significant effect of medication on these domains, although there was a medication effect detected for auditory event-related potentials. It is noteworthy that the use of covariate analyses may not always be the most sensitive means of evaluating potential medication effects because medication use itself may covary with other clinically relevant characteristics such as severity of illness, which is not taken into account in this type of analysis. In addition, some of the studies, most of which had small sample sizes, may have lacked the power to detect a potential medication effect. This is particularly relevant for the studies discussed above that examined PTSD patients who were taking benzodiazepines or antidepressants at the time of the study, because it has been clearly demonstrated that these drugs can affect memory performance17,18 as well as emotional processing.19 In addition to memory, psychomotor functioning and concentration are the cognitive domains most commonly affected by commonly used prescription medications.20 Furthermore, complex interactions between medication and task performance need to be considered, because some medication types may influence performance of certain tasks. To date, it is impossible to determine which medication types will influence performance of certain tasks by PTSD patients, because these influences have not directly been studied in this population. Therefore, differential interactions between medication type and task need to be taken into account when studying BOLD responses in PTSD patients. This warrants separate analysis of patients using only one type of pharmacologic agent as well as an evaluation of the potential differential effects of various psychotropic agents. To our knowledge, no PTSD neuroimaging studies have manipulated medication status and compared task performance independent of PTSD symptoms. In addition, only a few studies have examined the effects of psychotropic medications on CBF; these are described in the next section. ### Direct effects of psychotropic medications on CBF in healthy participants Understanding the effects of psychotropic medications on CBF in brain areas associated with PTSD is important for determining the potential effects of including medicated patients in neuroimaging studies.21 Current psychobiologic models construe PTSD fundamentally as a disorder of fear and affect arousal regulation,22–25 and they implicate alterations in the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex and hippocampus. Studies have begun to examine the effects of several psychotropic medications on CBF in these brain areas in healthy participants, including SSRIs, benzodiazepines, methamphetamine, ketamine and lamotrigine (Table 1). The SSRIs and benzodiazepines are particularly relevant to PTSD because they are often used in the treatment of this condition. View this table: [Table 1](http://jpn.ca/content/35/2/80/T1) Table 1 Effects of psychotropic agents on brain function in healthy controls During a hand sensorimotor task, a single dose of the SSRI fluoxetine and fenozolone led to more focused activation in the contralateral sensorimotor area, increased activation of the posterior supplementary motor area and a widespread decrease in bilateral cerebellar activation, as assessed by fMRI in healthy controls.26,27 Moreover, 1-month administration of paroxetine modulated human motor cortex excitability in healthy participants.27 More recently, Peran and colleagues28 examined the effects of long-term paroxetine administration (20 mg for 1 mo) on the activity of cortical areas implicated in higher-order representations of action-related language processing in healthy controls. Both verb generation and mental simulation of action showed a paroxetine-mediated hypoactivation of the left prefrontal and right medial premotor cortex and hyperactivation of the right Brodmann area (BA) 6 for less demanding verb repetition tasks. Short-term administration of citalopram (1 dose of 7.5 mg given intravenously) in healthy volunteers enhanced the right BA 47 responses and attenuated the medial orbitofrontal cortex to the no-go condition. In contrast, citalopram attenuated right BA 47 responses but attenuated right amygdala response to aversive faces.29 Recently, this finding has been replicated in a randomized, placebo-controlled study.30 Healthy volunteers were given either a single oral dose of citalopram (20 mg) or placebo. Amygdala activity in reaction to fearful facial expressions was significantly reduced 3 hours later in the treatment group compared with the placebo group. A similar effect was found for long-term treatment with escitalopram for 21 days,31 which led to a reduction in amygdala activation during an emotion face assessment task.31 This was also found in a placebo-controlled 7-day trial32 that used nonconscious exposure to fearful faces. These effects of SSRIs on amygdala responses to threat support the idea that antide-pressants may produce relevant effects in emotion processing areas early in the course of treatment. The amygdala is known to be involved in the processing of emotional stimuli and, in particular, the rapid detection of threat-relevant cues such as fearful facial expressions.33 Because both depression and PTSD have been associated with hyperactivity of the amygdala,4,34,35,36 and converging evidence demonstrates that one mechanism by which SSRIs may exert their action is by constraining such overactivity, the effects of SSRIs on BOLD responses in paradigms examining threat-relevant cues in PTSD should be studied. Therefore, patients taking SSRIs should be included in PTSD studies. The effects of short-term treatment with the selective nora-drenaline reuptake inhibitor reboxetine were also investigated in healthy volunteers during processing of emotional faces. Compared with those taking a placebo, those taking reboxetine had a reduced amygdala response to fearful faces and increased activation to happy versus neutral facial expressions in the right fusiform gyrus.37 The effects of benzodiazepines on brain activation during memory encoding tasks have been investigated.38,39 Studies using PET revealed that triazolam significantly impaired memory performance and led to deactivation in brain areas previously shown to be associated with memory encoding, including the anterior cingulate cortex, precuneus and cerebellum.38 These results were later replicated in a similar study which found that triazolam produced dose-related impairment in memory performance and dose-related deactivation in brain regions previously shown to be associated with memory encoding and known to show differential activity or connectivity in PTSD, such as the anterior cingulate cortex, prefrontal cortex and parahippocampal gyrus.39 Dose-dependent decreases in brain activation in the bilateral amygdala and insula have also been reported in response to lorazepam (1-time dosing) as compared with placebo during an emotion face assessment task in healthy volunteers,40 rendering these studies equally relevant for PTSD research. The effects of methamphetamine on cerebral activity have been examined using dynamic gradient-echo imaging before, during and after intravenous administration in healthy controls. Methamphetamine was associated with fMRI signal increases in the cerebellum and subcortical grey matter structures.41 In healthy participants, ketamine has been shown to be associated with CBF increases in the anterior cingulate, medial prefrontal and inferior frontal cortices and decreases in cerebral activation in the cerebellum.42 In this study, brain activation in the anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex was correlated with the psychomimetic action of ketamine, whereas there was no correlation with this psychologic change and the level of brain activation in the cerebellum. The consequences of ketamine and placebo and their effects on BOLD versus alterations in cognitive state in healthy participants has also been examined.43 The changes in BOLD signal during perception of the face–nonface contrast exhibited widespread BOLD changes. However, in the placebo group compared with in the ketamine group, there was significantly greater BOLD signal change only in the middle occipital gyrus (BA 18) and the precentral gyrus (BA 4). The changes in BOLD signal were not significantly greater in any region during ketamine administration. The authors suggested that the BOLD signal changes represented task-dependent effects of drug or placebo, rather than task-independent effects of drug.43 The effects of lamotrigine on BOLD signal changes induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) have also been examined in healthy participants.44 Lamotrigine at clinically relevant serum concentrations diffusely inhibited cortical activation induced by TMS applied over the motor cortex. In contrast, when TMS was applied over the prefrontal cortex, lamotrigine increased the TMS-induced activation of limbic regions such as the orbitofrontal cortex and hippocampus. In summary, there is sufficient evidence that psychotropic drugs can influence task-dependent and task-independent cerebral blood flow in healthy participants. Additionally, several studies have shown similar effects in other psychiatric populations such as schizophrenia, depression and bipolar disorder. Schlagenhauf and colleagues state that both typical neuroleptics and olanzapine influence task-dependent blood flow in schizophrenia patients using BOLD responses during a working memory task.45 In a longitudinal study, schizophrenia patients exhibited significant correlations between the increase in the LORETA value of the left superior temporal gyrus during an auditory oddball task with improvements of negative symptoms after 6-month treatment with olanzapine.46 In patients with depression, functional correlates of antidepressant treatment and symptomatic responses were identified using pre–post comparisons of activations elicited by sad facial affect processing,47 masked emotional faces,48 working memory tasks49 and in combination with sleep deprivation.50 In bipolar disorder, unmedicated patients exhibited significantly higher BOLD responses throughout the motor cortex, basal ganglia and thalamus compared with patients taking antipsychotic or mood-stabilizing medication during a motor reaction task.51 In addition, a study examining emotion processing demonstrated decreased amygdala activity in medicated versus unmedicated bipolar patients.52,53 These findings indicate that careful studies examining medication effects on neuroimaging results are necessary to analyze possible interaction effects between disorder and medication type. Therefore, careful examination of specific medication effects in PTSD will be essential. ### Preliminary evidence of medication effects in PTSD Several studies have begun to examine the differential effects of psychotropic drugs on brain function in PTSD. For example, investigators have examined the effects of yohimbine, an α2 adrenoreceptor antagonist, on cerebral glucose metabolism in combat-related PTSD. The veterans with PTSD exhibited increased anxiety in response to yohimbine and decreased metabolism in the prefrontal, temporal, parietal and orbitofrontal cortex compared with healthy participants.54 In a group of 11 patients with PTSD, treatment with citalopram was associated with decreased activation in the left medial temporal cortex, and symptomatic improvement was positively correlated with increased medial prefrontal cortex activity.55 Whereas the effects of long-term treatment with psycho -pharmacologic agents on brain morphology have been investigated extensively (e.g., in schizophrenia56), literature on neuroanatomical changes associated with pharmacotherapy in PTSD is just beginning to emerge. The neurobiology of PTSD has been hypothesized to involve alterations in gluta-matergic transmission with subsequent neurotoxicity, especially involving the hippocampal region. Phenytoin, an anticonvulsant used to treat epilepsy, is believed to act by modulating glutamatergic neurotransmission and has been associated with a 6% increase in right brain volume. Moreover, increased hippocampal volume was correlated with reductions of PTSD symptom severity.57 Animal studies have suggested that stress is associated with hippocampal volume changes, inhibition of neurogenesis and deficits in hippocampal-based memory dysfunction, and the literature is beginning to show that SSRIs promote neurogenesis and can reverse the effects of stress on hippocampal atrophy, whereas the neuro-biological mediators are still being investigated.58 In PTSD patients, treatment with the serotonin-specific reuptake blocker paroxetine for 1 year resulted in a 5% increase in hippocampal volume and a 35% improvement in declarative memory function.59 In conclusion, there is evidence in healthy participants that some psychotropic drugs not commonly used to treat PTSD, including methamphetamine, ketamine and clozapine, may affect brain regions that have been implicated in PTSD, including the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex. Accumulating evidence suggests that some SSRIs and lamotrigine affect some of these same regions. Benzodiazepines have clearly been shown to affect memory performance and brain activation of regions that have been associated with PTSD, including the anterior cingulate cortex, prefrontal cortex and parahippocampal gyrus. It is of note that most of the above described studies were conducted in non-PTSD samples and across a variety of paradigms. As a result, the findings can only be generalized with caution to PTSD neuroimaging studies because the effects of psychotropic drugs on CBF in PTSD remain largely unknown. Given the emerging evidence of differential effects of various psychotropic agents on CBF and/or BOLD, future studies need to continue to focus on evaluating the specific effects of each psychotropic agent in healthy populations and patients with PTSD. Furthermore, no studies to date have examined the effects of adjunctive pharmacotherapeutic treatment in PTSD (even though multiple medications are often used in the treatment of PTSD60 or the extent of placebo effects in this population.61,62 Because a positive placebo response is evident in up to 50% of patients with pain syndromes and depression, the influence of psychopharmacologic agents in PTSD patients should be explored in randomized, placebo-controlled trials. ### Disadvantages of including medication-free patients One disadvantage of focusing purely on medication-free PTSD patients is the risk of studying a population that does not necessarily represent all patients with PTSD. This is not such an issue if the aim is to isolate underlying mechanisms of the disorder; however, it is still important to establish whether such mechanisms can be equally well identified in patients encountered in routine clinical practice. Even though to date no studies have compared characteristics of treated and untreated PTSD patients, future studies should focus on factors that may prevent PTSD patients from seeking treatment. Several studies have examined prescribing practices in a variety of PTSD populations and have reported that 77% and 80% of domestic and military PTSD populations, respectively, received psychotropic medications,60,63 suggesting that medication-free PTSD patients do not represent the majority of PTSD patients. In addition to the implications of representativeness, including only medication-free patients in PTSD research may also introduce various sample biases. Samples are likely to include patients and treating psychiatrists who can be persuaded to voluntarily discontinue medication. In many cases these patients may be less severely ill or less prone to have their anxiety exacerbated by being exposed to the scanning environment.64–66 This is particularly problematic because PTSD patients with more severe and complex symptoms are often much more difficult to treat and therefore present a greater financial burden to the health care system.67,68 Understanding the pathophysiology of PTSD may contribute to the development of better treatment strategies; however, if the pathophysiology differs in patients with different disease severity, the patient groups excluded from research are less likely to benefit from these advances. In fact, investigators of other psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and mood disorders have a long history of performing neuroimaging research with both unmedicated and medicated patients for this reason.69,70 An additional disadvantage of including PTSD patients who have undergone a drug washout is that this procedure and its associated potential withdrawal symptoms, especially related to the use of antidepressant medications, may have independent effects on CBF. For example, proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy has shown that selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor discontinuation syndrome is associated with a rostral anterior cingulate choline metabolite decrease.71 Drug washouts may therefore introduce rather than eliminate a potential confounding factor. There are few neuro-imaging studies that have examined the potential effects of psychotropic drug washouts and/or withdrawal symptoms on CBF, which therefore remain largely unknown. One must also ask whether there are ethical issues raised by a protocol that involves weaning severely ill patients off any intervention, including drugs, when that intervention has been shown to be beneficial for that individual. In other conditions for which drugs have a more established role, such as schizophrenia, many institutional review boards are taking the view that this poses an unacceptable level of risk. If this view becomes commonly accepted in relation to PTSD studies, or in relation to a subset of PTSD patients, an evidence base that is restricted to unmedicated patients will need broadening to advance the field. An additional option to address this issue is to use study designs that include patients before medication treatment whenever possible. ### Should neuroimaging studies include PTSD patients receiving psychotherapy? To date, PTSD neuroimaging studies have excluded medicated PTSD patients, yet many have not excluded patients receiving psychotherapeutic interventions. This point is important in light of the increasing evidence that psychotherapeutic interventions can have profound effects on CBF. For example, in a sample of subthreshold PTSD patients who were compared with matched waiting-list subthreshold PTSD patients, a course of exposure-based and cognitive restructuring therapy led to significantly higher levels of CBF in the parietal lobes, left hippocampus, thalamus and left prefrontal cortex during memory retrieval after psychotherapy.72 In addition, the effects of eye movement desensitization and reprocessing therapy on CBF in a group of PTSD patients listening to a traumatic script showed that a significant tracer difference present before treatment in the uncus (BA 36) disappeared after treatment, while a significant difference appeared in the lateral temporal pole (BA 21).73 Furthermore, in a group of 8 patients with PTSD, imaginal exposure and cognitive restructuring was associated with increased activation in the rostral anterior cingulate cortex,74 and improvements in PTSD symptom severity were correlated with increased rostral anterior cingulate activation and decreased amygdala activation. The effects of brief eclectic therapy involving trauma imagery in civilian PTSD patients were associated with decreased CBF in the right middle frontal gyrus as determined by PET. Treatment effects correlated positively with activation in the left superior temporal gyrus and superior/middle frontal gyrus.75 Research has also shown the effects of psychotherapeutic intervention on CBF in depression,76 obsessive–compulsive disorder,77 panic disorder,78 social phobia79 and spider phobia.80,81 In unipolar depression,82 a prospective PET study revealed comparative results for psychotherapeutic (cognitive behavioural therapy) and pharmacologic (venlafaxine) treatment. Both treatments yielded a significant increase in glucose metabolism in the right occipital-temporal cortex and significant decreases in the orbitofrontal cortex and left medial pre-frontal cortex. Patients who responded to either or both treatments were characterized by reduced metabolism in several prefrontal regions. In light of these results, the different inclusion standards for patients treated with psychopharmacologic agents versus psychotherapy seem anachronistic. In summary, there appears to be compelling evidence for the potentially confounding effects on brain areas relevant to PTSD for psychotherapeutic interventions. Given these results, it appears that the effects of psychotherapy, just like those of pharmacotherapy, may confound the results of neuroimaging studies. It may therefore be important to note whether patients are currently engaged in psychotherapeutic treatment and include such information in the analyses of neuroimaging data. ## Conclusion The general principle of including medication-free psychiatric patients whenever possible is widely considered a desirable factor and may play an important role in decisions concerning the funding and publication of neuroimaging studies. At least in PTSD, however, the empirical evidence for relevant medications having a confounding effect on task performance or CBF in relevant areas remains sparse for most psychotropic medications because detailed dismantling studies are lacking. However, considerable evidence is accumulating in healthy controls for 2 of the most commonly prescribed medication classes, SSRIs and benzodiazepines. We believe that the field will best be advanced by studies that can speak to whether findings from medication-free participants can be meaningfully applied to the actual patient population, as well as by knowing how or which medications affect which outcomes. Changing approaches to research participant protection may mean that, at some point, studying medication-free patients may become very difficult (unless patients can be accessed and studied before receiving treatment), rendering the need to investigate medication effects on neuroimaging data particularly urgent. Given the rather substantial evidence for the effects of psychotherapy on brain function, there are also likely to be considerable benefits from comparing the separate effects of psychologic and pharmacologic intervention. There are a number of alternative strategies that do not require discontinuing medication to remove potential confounding effects. Within-patient designs have the effect of holding medication status constant while manipulating the conditions of interest. Other studies have attempted to control for medication status by having a psychiatric control group that is receiving similar pharmacologic treatment. In the case of PTSD, patients with major depressive disorder may sometimes provide an appropriate comparison group,83 although the possibility of a drug-by-disorder interaction cannot be excluded. Furthermore, neurobiologic changes associated with the preventive prescription of psychopharmaceutical agents in acutely traumatized patients have emerged as a topic of interest. To date, studies prospectively assessing symptom severity have yielded mixed results, indicating a need for further study.84–87 Whereas the effects of long-term treatment with psychopharmacologic agents on brain morphology have been investigated in other psychiatric disorders, neuro-anatomical changes associated with short- and long-term pharmacotherapy in PTSD deserve further investigation. In addition, there are a number of testable hypotheses addressing medication effects in PTSD patients and healthy controls include. The first hypothesis is that the effects of short- and long-term administration of psychotropic medications on neural functioning during a resting state differ in PTSD patients and healthy controls. The second is that the effects of psychotropic medications on neural functioning at baseline versus during relevant cognitive tasks, such as working memory tasks,88 differ in PTSD patients and healthy controls. Such studies will allow an examination of brain activation changes associated with task-dependent effects of the drug or placebo and task-independent effects of the drug or placebo. The third hypothesis is that the effects of psycho-tropic medications differ under conditions that directly trigger PTSD symptoms such as the script-driven imagery symptom provocation paradigm as compared with baseline conditions. Such studies will enable an increased understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying medication and placebo effects on specific PTSD symptoms. Finally, to gain further insight into the underlying mechanisms of pharmacotherapeutic agents often used adjunctively in PTSD, one may determine the adjunctive effects of drug A by randomly assigning PTSD patients who are receiving drug B to also receive either drug A or placebo. In conclusion, we make the following recommendations with regard to future research directions and the inclusion of PTSD patients currently taking psychotropic medications in neuroimaging studies. First, we agree with the findings of a recent review of similar issues in bipolar disorder,70 which concluded that neuroimaging studies should ideally recruit both medicated and unmedicated patients to allow the findings to be generalized with greater confidence to the entire population of PTSD patients. Furthermore, more research is clearly needed on the independent effects of medications on task performance and on blood flow in regions of interest in PTSD. Therefore, separate and comparative analyses of medicated and unmedicated patients that carefully differentiate the effects of symptom severity, task difficulty and medication influence are called for. In addition, neuroimaging studies should take account of whether patients are currently engaged in psychotherapeutic treatment and include such information in the data analyses. Future research focusing on these different areas of investigation should not only provide greater insight into the mechanisms underlying PTSD, but also shed more light on the specific effects of psychotropic medications on brain functioning in PTSD. Because pharmacologically induced changes in BOLD signal could reflect influences not only on neural activity but also on the synaptic and metabolic signalling to the blood vessels that control the cerebral blood flow responses, studies using fMRI to investigate the effects of psychopharmaceutical agents should consider whether pharma-induced alterations of the BOLD signal reflect influences on neurovascular, rather than neural, activity. To control for nonspecific neurovascular alterations, the inclusion of a control task to assess the BOLD response in brain regions not expected to be modulated by the drug is recommended. Randomized, placebo-controlled studies that use different tasks expected to show differential activity in PTSD patients would not only allow the determination of whether the psychopharmacologic medication affects task performance but also might serve to substantiate psychobiologic models of PTSD. ## Acknowledgements The authors gratefully acknowledge advice and suggestions by Roger K. Pitman. The authors thank Nancy Mazza for helping prepare the manuscript for publication. ## Footnotes * **Competing interests:** None declared for Drs. Lanius, Brewin, Bremner, Daniels, Liberzon, McFarlane, Schnurr, Shin, Stein and Vermetten. Dr. Friedman received speaker fees from AstraZeneca in 2008. * **Contributors:** Drs. Brewin, Friedman, Liberzon, Schnurr, Shin and Stein designed the study. Drs. Lanius and Liberzon acquired the data, which Drs. Lanius, Daniels, Friedman, McFarlane, Schnurr, Shin, Stein and Vermetten analyzed. Drs. Lanius, Brewin, Daniels, Friedman, Liberzon and Stein wrote the article. All authors reviewed the article and approved the final version submitted for publication. * Received April 16, 2009. * Revision received September 3, 2009. * Accepted October 16, 2009. ## References 1. Etkin A, Wager TD.Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia.Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:1476–88. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17898336&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000250049600008&link_type=ISI) 2. Francati V, Vermetten E, Bremner JD.Functional neuroimaging studies in posttraumatic stress disorder: review of current methods and findings.Depress Anxiety 2007;24:202–18. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/da.20208&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16960853&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000246250300007&link_type=ISI) 3. Damsa C, Kosel M, Moussally J.Current status of brain imaging in anxiety disorders.Curr Opin Psychiatry 2009;22:96–110. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/YCO.0b013e328319bd10&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19122541&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000262398400017&link_type=ISI) 4. Shin LM, Liberzon I.The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders.Neuropsychopharmacology 2010;35:169–91. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/npp.2009.83&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19625997&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000272784600010&link_type=ISI) 5. Orr SP, Solomon Z, Peri T, et al.Physiologic responses to loud tones in Israeli veterans of the 1973 Yom Kippur War.Biol Psychiatry 1997;41:319–26. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(95)00671-0&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9024955&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1997WF21800009&link_type=ISI) 6. Orr SP, Metzger LJ, Lasko NB, et al.Physiologic responses to sudden, loud tones in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: association with posttraumatic stress disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60:283–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.60.3.283&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12622661&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000181572200008&link_type=ISI) 7. Milad MR, Orr SP, Lasko NB, et al.Presence and acquired origin of reduced recall for fear extinction in PTSD: results of a twin study.J Psychiatr Res 2008;42:515–20. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.01.017&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18313695&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000255424000001&link_type=ISI) 8. Elman I, Ariely D, Mazar N, et al.Probing reward function in post-traumatic stress disorder with beautiful facial images.Psychiatry Res 2005;135:179–83. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.psychres.2005.04.002&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15993948&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000230969600002&link_type=ISI) 9. Gilbertson MW, Paulus LA, Williston SK, et al.Neurocognitive function in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: relationship to posttraumatic stress disorder.J Abnorm Psychol 2006;115:484–95. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.484&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16866589&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000239369300010&link_type=ISI) 10. Vasterling JJ, Duke LM, Brailey K, et al.Attention, learning, and memory performances and intellectual resources in Vietnam veterans: PTSD and no disorder comparisons.Neuropsychology 2002;16:5–14. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0894-4105.16.1.5&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11853357&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000173794700002&link_type=ISI) 11. Golier JA, Yehuda R, Lupien SJ, et al.Memory performance in Holocaust survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder.Am J Psychiatry 2002;159:1682–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1682&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12359673&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000178382700011&link_type=ISI) 12. Nixon RD, Nishith P, Resick PA.The accumulative effect of trauma exposure on short-term and delayed verbal memory in a treatment-seeking sample of female rape victims.J Trauma Stress 2004;17:31–5. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014673.02925.db&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15027790&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000188745900004&link_type=ISI) 13. Orr SP, Lasko NB, Metzger LJ, et al.Psychophysiologic assessment of women with posttraumatic stress disorder resulting from childhood sexual abuse.J Consult Clin Psychol 1998;66:906–13. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0022-006X.66.6.906&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9874903&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000077706100003&link_type=ISI) 14. Vasterling JJ, Brailey K, Sutker PB.Olfactory identification in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.J Trauma Stress 2000;13:241–53. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1023/A:1007754611030&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10838673&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000087186700005&link_type=ISI) 15. Vasterling JJ, Rogers C, Kaplan E.Qualitative block design analysis in posttraumatic stress disorder.Assessment 2000;7:217–26. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/107319110000700302&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11037389&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 16. Metzger LJ, Orr SP, Lasko NB, et al.Auditory event-related potentials to tone stimuli in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.Biol Psychiatry 1997;42:1006–15. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00138-8&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9386852&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 17. Mintzer MZ, Griffiths RR.Differential effects of scopolamine and lorazepam on working memory maintenance versus manipulation processes.Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2007;7:120–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.3758/CABN.7.2.120&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17672383&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 18. Mirow S.Cognitive dysfunction associated with fluoxetine.Am J Psychiatry 1991;148:948–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/ajp.148.7.948&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=2053640&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1991FU83600035&link_type=ISI) 19. Murphy SE, Downham C, Cowen PJ, et al.Direct effects of diazepam on emotional processing in healthy volunteers.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;199:503–13. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s00213-008-1082-2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18581100&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 20. Stein RA, Strickland TL.A review of the neuropsychological effects of commonly used prescription medications.Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1998;13:259–84. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00027-9&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14590642&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000073008100002&link_type=ISI) 21. Kilts C.In vivo neuroimaging correlates of the efficacy of paroxetine in the treatment of mood and anxiety disorders.Psychopharmacol Bull 2003;37 Suppl 119–28. 22. Shin LM, Rauch SL, Pitman RK.Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampal function in PTSD.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2006;1071:67–79. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1196/annals.1364.007&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16891563&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000240653600007&link_type=ISI) 23. Frewen PA, Lanius RA.Toward a psychobiology of posttraumatic self-dysregulation: reexperiencing, hyperarousal, dissociation, and emotional numbing.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2006;1071:110–24. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1196/annals.1364.010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16891566&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000240653600010&link_type=ISI) 24. Liberzon I, Sripada CS.The functional neuroanatomy of PTSD: a critical review.Prog Brain Res 2008;167:151–69. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0079-6123(07)67011-3&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18037013&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000252487200011&link_type=ISI) 25. Liberzon I, Martis B.Neuroimaging studies of emotional responses in PTSD.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2006;1071:87–109. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1196/annals.1364.009&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16891565&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000240653600009&link_type=ISI) 26. Loubinoux I, Boulanouar K, Ranjeva JP, et al.Cerebral functional magnetic resonance imaging activation modulated by a single dose of the monoamine neurotransmission enhancers fluoxetine and fenozolone during hand sensorimotor tasks.J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1999;19:1365–75. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/00004647-199912000-00010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10598941&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000084884200010&link_type=ISI) 27. Loubinoux I, Tombari D, Pariente J, et al.Modulation of behavior and cortical motor activity in healthy subjects by a chronic administration of a serotonin enhancer.Neuroimage 2005;27:299–313. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.023&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15950492&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 28. Peran P, Demonet JF, Cardebat D.Paroxetine-induced modulation of cortical activity supporting language representations of action.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;195:487–96. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17874333&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 29. Del-Ben CM, Deakin JF, McKie S, et al.The effect of citalopram pretreatment on neuronal responses to neuropsychological tasks in normal volunteers: an FMRI study.Neuropsychopharmacology 2005;30:1724–34. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/sj.npp.1300728&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15827569&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000231264000015&link_type=ISI) 30. Murphy SE, Norbury R, O’Sullivan U, et al.Effect of a single dose of citalopram on amygdala response to emotional faces.Br J Psychiatry 2009;194:535–40. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6ImJqcHJjcHN5Y2giO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6OToiMTk0LzYvNTM1IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTc6Ii9qcG4vMzUvMi84MC5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 31. Arce E, Simmons AN, Lovero KL, et al.Escitalopram effects on in-sula and amygdale BOLD activation during emotional processing.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;196:661–72. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s00213-007-1004-8&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18058090&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 32. Harmer CJ, Mackay CE, Reid CB, et al.Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues.Biol Psychiatry 2006;59:816–20. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.015&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16460693&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000237049900006&link_type=ISI) 33. Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, et al.A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions.Nature 1996;383:812–5. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/383812a0&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8893004&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1996VQ14400060&link_type=ISI) 34. Peluso MA, Glahn DC, Matsuo K, et al.Amygdala hyperactivation in untreated depressed individuals.Psychiatry Res 2009;173:158–61. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.03.006&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19545982&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000269113000013&link_type=ISI) 35. Drevets WC.Neuroimaging abnormalities in the amygdala in mood disorders.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003;985:420–44. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07098.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12724175&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000182918800032&link_type=ISI) 36. Mayberg HS.Defining the neural circuitry of depression: toward a new nosology with therapeutic implications.Biol Psychiatry 2007;61:729–30. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.01.013&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17338903&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000245002700001&link_type=ISI) 37. Norbury R, Mackay CE.Short-term antidepressant treatment and facial processing: Functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Br J Psychiatry 2007;190:531–2. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6ImJqcHJjcHN5Y2giO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6OToiMTkwLzYvNTMxIjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTc6Ii9qcG4vMzUvMi84MC5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 38. Mintzer MZ, Griffiths RR, Contoreggi C, et al.Effects of triazolam on brain activity during episodic memory encoding: a PET study.Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;25:744–56. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00280-9&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11682258&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000171952200015&link_type=ISI) 39. Mintzer MZ, Kuwabara H, Alexander M, et al.Dose effects of triazolam on brain activity during episodic memory encoding: a PET Study.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;188:445–61. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s00213-006-0446-8&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16847681&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 40. Paulus MP, Feinstein JS, Castillo G, et al.Dose-dependent decrease of activation in bilateral amygdale and insula by lorazepam during emotion processing.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62:282–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.282&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15753241&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000227391600006&link_type=ISI) 41. Kleinschmidt A, Bruhm H, Kruger G, et al.Effects of sedation, stimulation, and placebo on cerebral blood oxygenation: a magnetic resonance neuroimaging study of psychotropic drug action.NMR Biomed 1999;12:286–92. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/(SICI)1099-1492(199908)12:5<286::AID-NBM576>3.0.CO;2-Q&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10484817&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000082499400003&link_type=ISI) 42. Holcomb HH, Lahti AC, Medoff DR, et al.Sequential regional cerebral blood flow brain scans using PET with H2(15)O demonstrate ketamine actions in CNS dynamically.Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;25:165–72. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00229-9&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11425500&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000170653800002&link_type=ISI) 43. Abel KM, Allin MP, Kucharska-Pietura K, et al.Ketamine and fMRI BOLD signal: distinguishing between effects mediated by change in blood flow versus change in cognitive state.Hum Brain Mapp 2003;18:135–45. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/hbm.10064&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12518293&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000180635400006&link_type=ISI) 44. Li X, Teneback CC, Nahas Z, et al.Interleaved transcranial magnetic stimulation/functional MRI confirms that lamotrigine inhibits cortical excitability in healthy young men.Neuropsychopharmacology 2004;29:1395–407. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/sj.npp.1300452&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15100699&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000222110700019&link_type=ISI) 45. Schlagenhauf F, Wüstenberg T, Schmack K, et al.Switching schizophrenia patients from typical neuroleptics to olanzapine: effects on BOLD response during attention and working memory.Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2008;18:589–99. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.04.013&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18554874&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000258217400006&link_type=ISI) 46. Higuchi Y, Sumiyoshi T, Kawasaki Y, et al.Electrophysiological basis for the ability of olanzapine to improve verbal memory and functional outcome in patients with schizophrenia: a LORETA analysis of P300.Schizophr Res 2008;101:320–30. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18321680&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 47. Fu CH, Williams SC, Cleare AJ, et al.Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61:877–89. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.877&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15351766&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000223726200003&link_type=ISI) 48. Sheline YI, Barch DM, Donnelly JM, et al.Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study.Biol Psychiatry 2001;50:651–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01263-X&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11704071&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000172035100001&link_type=ISI) 49. Walsh ND, Williams SC, Brammer MJ, et al.A longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study of verbal working memory in depression after antidepressant therapy.Biol Psychiatry 2007;62:1236–43. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.022&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17601497&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000251119600006&link_type=ISI) 50. Smith GS, Reynolds CFR, Houck PR, et al.Cerebral glucose metabolic response to combined total sleep deprivation and antidepressant treatment in geriatric depression: a randomized, placebo-controlled study.Psychiatry Res 2009;171:1–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.05.001&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19087899&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000262942500001&link_type=ISI) 51. Caligiuri MP, Brown GG, Meloy MJ, et al.An fMRI study of affective state and medication on cortical and subcortical brain regions during motor performance in bipolar disorder.Psychiatry Res 2003;123:171–82. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00098-2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12928105&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000184973700003&link_type=ISI) 52. Blumberg HP, Donegan NH, Sanislow CA, et al.Preliminary evidence for medication effects on functional abnormalities in the amygdala and anterior cingulate in bipolar disorder.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:308–13. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s00213-005-0156-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16249909&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 53. Keener MT, Phillips ML.Neuroimaging in bipolar disorder: a critical review of current findings.Curr Psychiatry Rep 2007;9:512–20. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s11920-007-0070-2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18221633&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 54. Bremner JD, Innis RB, Ng CK, et al.Positron emission tomography measurement of cerebral metabolic correlates of yohimbine administration in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54:246–54. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830150070011&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9075465&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1997WP35500007&link_type=ISI) 55. Seedat S, Warwick J, van Heerden B, et al.Single photon emission computed tomography in posttraumatic stress disorder before and after treatment with a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor.J Affect Disord 2004;80:45–53. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00047-8&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15094257&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000221147500006&link_type=ISI) 56. Crespo-Facorro B, Roiz-Santiáñez R, Pérez-Iglesias R, et al.Effect of antipsychotic drugs on brain morphometry. A randomized controlled one-year follow-up study of haloperidol, risperidone and olanzapine.Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2008;32:1936–43. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.09.020&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18930104&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 57. Bremner JD, Mletzko T, Welter S, et al.Effects of phenytoin on memory, cognition and brain structure in post-traumatic stress disorder: a pilot study.J Psychopharmacol 2005;19:159–65. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/0269881105048996&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15728437&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000227735700009&link_type=ISI) 58. Mathew S, Price R, Shungu D, et al.A pilot study of the effects of chronic paroxetine administration on hippocampal N-acetylaspartate in generalized anxiety disorder.J Psychopharmacol 2009Feb9[Epub ahead of print] 59. Vermetten E, Vythilingam M, Southwick SM, et al.Long-term treatment with paroxetine increases verbal declarative memory and hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2003;54:693–702. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00634-6&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14512209&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000185500500005&link_type=ISI) 60. Mohamed S, Rosenheck RA.Pharmacotherapy of PTSD in the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs: diagnostic- and symptom-guided drug selection.J Clin Psychiatry 2008;69:959–65. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.4088/JCP.v69n0611&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18588361&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000257092800011&link_type=ISI) 61. Diederich N J, Goetz CG.The placebo treatments in neurosciences: new insights from clinical and neuroimaging studies.Neurology 2008;71:677–84. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1212/01.wnl.0000324635.49971.3d&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18725593&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 62. Zubieta JK, Stohler CS.Neurobiological mechanisms of placebo responses.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1156:198–210. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04424.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19338509&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000265827700010&link_type=ISI) 63. Mellman TA, Clark RE, Peacock WJ.Prescribing patterns for patients with posttraumatic stress disorder.Psychiatr Serv 2003;54:1618–21. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ps.54.12.1618&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14645801&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000222759200014&link_type=ISI) 64. Dewey M, Schink T, Dewey CF.Claustrophobia during magnetic resonance imaging: cohort study in over 55,000 patients.J Magn Reson Imaging 2007;26:1322–7. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/jmri.21147&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17969166&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 65. Eshed I, Althoff CE, Hamm B, et al.Claustrophobia and premature termination of magnetic resonance imaging examinations.J Magn Reson Imaging 2007;26:401–4. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/jmri.21012&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17610281&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000248577900022&link_type=ISI) 66. Grey SJ, Price G, Mathews A.Reduction of anxiety during MR imaging: a controlled trial.Magn Reson Imaging 2000;18:351–5. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0730-725X(00)00112-0&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10745145&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000086585100016&link_type=ISI) 67. Lanius RA, Tuhan I.Stage-oriented trauma treatment using dialectical behaviour therapy.Can J Psychiatry 2003;48:126–7. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/070674370304800212&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12655913&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 68. Spinazzola J, Blaustein M, van der Kolk BA.Posttraumatic stress disorder treatment outcome research: The study of unrepresentative samples?.J Trauma Stress 2005;18:425–36. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/jts.20050&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16281240&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000233446700006&link_type=ISI) 69. Rasetti R, Mattay VS, Wiedholz LM, et al.Evidence the altered amygdala activity in schizophrenia is related to clinical state and not genetic risk.Am J Psychiatry 2009;166:216–25. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08020261&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19074979&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000263031500015&link_type=ISI) 70. Phillips ML, Travis MJ, Fagiolini A, et al.Medication effects in neuroimaging studies of bipolar disorder.Am J Psychiatry 2008;165:313–20. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071066&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18245175&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000253779400008&link_type=ISI) 71. Kaufman MJ, Henry ME, Frederick B, et al.Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor discontinuation syndrome is associated with a rostral anterior cingulate choline metabolite decrease: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging study.Biol Psychiatry 2003;54:534–9. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12946882&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 72. Peres JF, Newberg AB, Mercante JP, et al.Cerebral blood flow changes during retrieval of traumatic memories before and after psychotherapy: a SPECT study.Psychol Med 2007;37:1481–91. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S003329170700997X&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17288648&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000250429800010&link_type=ISI) 73. Pagani M, Hogberg G, Salmaso D, et al.Effects of EMDR psychotherapy on 99mTc-HMPAO distribution in occupation-related post-traumatic stress disorder.Nucl Med Commun 2007;28:757–65. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/MNM.0b013e3282742035&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17728604&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 74. Felmingham K, Kemp A, Williams L, et al.Changes in anterior cingulate and amygdala after cognitive behavior therapy of posttraumatic stress disorder.Psychol Sci 2007;18:127–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01860.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17425531&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000245157900007&link_type=ISI) 75. Lindauer RJ, Booij J, Habraken JB, et al.Effects of psychotherapy on regional cerebral blood flow during trauma imagery in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder: a randomized clinical trial.Psychol Med 2008;38:543–54. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17803835&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 76. Goldapple K, Segal Z, Garson C, et al.Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61:34–41. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.61.1.34&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14706942&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000187894000004&link_type=ISI) 77. Baxter LR Jr., Schwartz JM, Bergman KS, et al.Caudate glucose metabolic rate changes with both drug and behavior therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992;49:681–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820090009002&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=1514872&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1992JM05200001&link_type=ISI) 78. Prasko J, Horacek J, Zalesky R, et al.The change of regional brain metabolism (18FDG PET) in panic disorder during the treatment with cognitive behavioral therapy or antidepressants.Neuroendocrinol Lett 2004;25:340–8. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15580167&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) 79. Furmark T, Tillfors M, Marteinsdottir I, et al.Common changes in cerebral blood flow in patients with social phobia treated with citalopram or cognitive-behavioral therapy.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;59:425–33. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.59.5.425&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11982446&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000175409800004&link_type=ISI) 80. Paquette V, Levesque J, Mensour B, et al.“Change the mind and you change the brain”: effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy on the neural correlates of spider phobia.Neuroimage 2003;18:401–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00030-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12595193&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000181182500019&link_type=ISI) 81. Straube T, Glauer M, Dilger S, et al.Effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy on brain activation in specific phobia.Neuroimage 2006;29:125–35. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.049&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16087353&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000234581400013&link_type=ISI) 82. Kennedy SH, Konarski JZ, Segal ZV, et al.Differences in brain glucose metabolism between responders to CBT and venlafaxine in a 16-week randomized controlled trial.Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:778–88. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.164.5.778&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17475737&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000246370300018&link_type=ISI) 83. Whalley MG, Rugg MD, Smith APR, et al.Incidental retrieval of emotional contexts in PTSD and depression: an fMRI study.Brain Cogn 2009;69:98–107. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.bandc.2008.05.008&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18614265&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000263133900012&link_type=ISI) 84. Pitman RK, Sanders KM, Zusman RM, et al.Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with propranolol.Biol Psychiatry 2002;51:189–92. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01279-3&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11822998&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000173570900008&link_type=ISI) 85. Brunet A, Orr SP, Tremblay J, et al.Effect of post-retrieval propranolol on psychophysiologic responding during subsequent script-driven traumatic imagery in post-traumatic stress disorder.J Psychiatr Res 2008;42:503–6. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.05.006&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17588604&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000254720300010&link_type=ISI) 86. Stein MB, Kerridge C, Dimsdale JE, et al.Pharmacotherapy to prevent PTSD: results from a randomized controlled proof-of-concept trial in physically injured patients.J Trauma Stress 2007;20:923–32. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/jts.20270&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18157888&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000251952900001&link_type=ISI) 87. McGhee LL, Maani CV, Garza TH, et al.The effect of propranolol on posttraumatic stress disorder in burned service members.J Burn Care Res 2009;30:92–7. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/BCR.0b013e3181921f51&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19060728&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000262388900013&link_type=ISI) 88. Moores KA, Clark CR, McFarlane AC, et al.Abnormal recruitment of working memory updating networks during maintenance of trauma-neutral information in post-traumatic stress disorder.Psychiatry Res 2008;163:156–70. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.08.011&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18455372&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F35%2F2%2F80.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000257581200007&link_type=ISI)