Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders ========================================================================================= * Andrée M. Cusi * Anthony Nazarov * Katherine Holshausen * Glenda M. MacQueen * Margaret C. McKinnon ## Abstract **Background:** This review integrates neuroimaging studies of 2 domains of social cognition — emotion comprehension and theory of mind (ToM) — in patients with major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder. The influence of key clinical and method variables on patterns of neural activation during social cognitive processing is also examined. **Methods:** Studies were identified using PsycINFO and PubMed (January 1967 to May 2011). The search terms were “fMRI,” “emotion comprehension,” “emotion perception,” “affect comprehension,” “affect perception,” “facial expression,” “prosody,” “theory of mind,” “mentalizing” and “empathy” in combination with “major depressive disorder,” “bipolar disorder,” “major depression,” “unipolar depression,” “clinical depression” and “mania.” **Results:** Taken together, neuroimaging studies of social cognition in patients with mood disorders reveal enhanced activation in limbic and emotion-related structures and attenuated activity within frontal regions associated with emotion regulation and higher cognitive functions. These results reveal an overall lack of inhibition by higher-order cognitive structures on limbic and emotion-related structures during social cognitive processing in patients with mood disorders. Critically, key variables, including illness burden, symptom severity, comorbidity, medication status and cognitive load may moderate this pattern of neural activation. **Limitations:** Studies that did not include control tasks or a comparator group were included in this review. **Conclusion:** Further work is needed to examine the contribution of key moderator variables and to further elucidate the neural networks underlying altered social cognition in patients with mood disorders. The neural networks underlying higher-order social cognitive processes, including empathy, remain unexplored in patients with mood disorders. ## Introduction Major depressive disorder (MDD) and bipolar disorder are associated with alterations in multiple domains, including interpersonal and social functioning.1–4 Recent studies have examined the nature of social impairment in patients with mood disorders through the lens of social cognition, involving the ability to understand and respond to the thoughts and feelings of others and thought central to successful social interactions.5,6 The goal of the present review was to examine the findings of neuroimaging studies concerning social cognitive processing in patients with MDD and bipolar disorder. Here, we focus on studies investigating 2 different but overlapping aspects of social cognition — emotion comprehension and theory of mind (ToM) — that have recently been investigated in patients with mood disorders. Emotion recognition refers to an individual’s ability to infer the emotional state of another from observable information, such as prosody and facial expression. By contrast, ToM has been defined as the ability to ascribe mental states, such as beliefs, desires and intentions, to oneself and others.7 Taken together, these theoretical definitions reveal substantial overlap and distinct processes involved in these key components of social cognition. For example, theoretical models propose that the ability to recognize another’s emotion is critical to ToM,8 with empirical evidence supporting this view.9–15 Hence, we included these 2 domains of social cognition in our review to provide a broad understanding of the neural correlates of social cognition in patients with mood disorders. Other domains of social cognition, including empathy and moral reasoning, remain unexplored in these patients. In healthy individuals, the neural basis of social cognition consists of a complex network of brain areas involved in cognitive and affective processes (for a recent review, see Adolphs16). Key regions that contribute to this network include prefrontal regions, such as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, an area involved in the regulation of emotion and reward evaluation,17 and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), an area implicated in higher-order cognitive processes, such as cognitive control and executive functioning.18,19 The anterior cingulate cortex, involved in conflict monitoring and integration of information to motivate behaviour, has also been implicated.16 The amygdala, which is critical to processing and evaluation of emotional stimuli, and the ventral striatum, which is involved in emotional and motivational aspects of behaviour, also constitute important nodes in the social cognition network.5 Finally, temporal regions, such as the temporoparietal junction, which is involved in a sense of agency as well as perspective-taking, and the temporal poles, which are implicated in diverse memory functions, have been shown to be recruited across various social cognitive tasks, including ToM, empathy and facial emotion processing.20–22 Critically, many of the same neural regions thought to mediate the cognitive (e.g., memory, executive functioning) and affective (e.g., emotion evaluation) processes necessary for social cognitive responding have been implicated in patients with bipolar disorder and MDD, showing altered metabolic functioning and/or structural abnormalities (for a recent review see Price and Drevets23). Coupled with growing evidence of cognitive and affective processing impairments among patients with MDD and bipolar disorder,20,24–28 we expected our review to reveal that patients with mood disorders show alterations in neuronal functioning during social cognitive processing. Specifically, we expected that individuals with MDD and bipolar disorder would show lowered activation in prefrontal areas involved in emotion regulation and higher-order cognitive processes and increased activity in subcortical and limbic regions implicated in emotion appraisal and generation, indicating a lack of inhibition of higher-order cognitive centres on limbic and emotion-related structures. Here, the neural basis of each of these social cognitive domains is briefly reviewed and is followed by a synopsis of relevant behavioural studies of patients with mood disorders. The neuroimaging literature concerning social cognition in patients with mood disorders is then summarized. Where possible, neuroimaging findings involving patients with mood disorders are contextualized based on several key variables, including illness burden (e.g., number of affective episodes), illness state (e.g., active v. remitted), medication status, age, cognitive load and comorbidity, thought to moderate cognitive performance in patients with mood disorders.29 Depending on data availability, results for patients with MDD and bipolar disorder are presented separately and are subsequently compared. We conclude the review by discussing future research directions and the clinical implications of alterations in the neural circuitry underlying social cognition with respect to improving treatment interventions and preventing relapse in patients with mood disorders. On balance, patients with mood disorders experience substantial disruptions in interpersonal function,30–34 and deficits in social cognition may underlie these difficulties. An enhanced understanding of the neural correlates of social cognition may help improve diagnostic accuracy and early intervention among patients with these disorders. This is particularly pertinent to the study of bipolar disorder, given that patients with this illness experience depression more than hypomania or mania,35,36 which may result in the mis-diagnosis of bipolar depression as MDD,37,38 suboptimal treatment and poor outcome.39,40 Elucidating the neural correlates of social cognition may also assist in identifying the neuroanatomical basis of vulnerability to these disorders. Improved knowledge of the underlying neural mechanisms and nature of social cognitive deficits in patients with mood disorders may also assist in clarifying the nature of social dysfunction in patients with MDD and bipolar disorder and may aid in the development of psychologic interventions aimed at improving social perception and adjustment in these patient populations. ## Methods A search of the literature using PsycINFO and PubMed (from January 1967 to the second week of May, 2011) was conducted using the following key words: “fMRI,” “emotion comprehension,” “affect comprehension,” “emotion perception,” “affect perception,” “facial expression,” “prosody,” “theory of mind” and “mentalizing” in combination with the diagnostic terms “major depressive disorder,” “major depression,” “unipolar depression,” “clinical depression,” “bipolar disorder” and “mania.” Reference lists of retrieved articles were also manually searched for relevant publications. All neuroimaging articles investigating the perception of affective stimuli (facial expressions and prosody), ToM and empathic responding in patients with a diagnosis of MDD or bipolar disorder were evaluated. Given the dearth of papers investigating the domains of ToM in patients with mood disorders, we adopted a liberal approach to the inclusion of relevant articles in these domains. Cross-sectional studies with both a patient sample and matched controls as well as longitudinal studies comprising only a patient sample were included. Only studies that involved an experimental task or survey measuring affective responding or ToM were included. Studies that recruited patients with a primary diagnosis of MDD or bipolar disorder and those that used recognized diagnostic criteria were included. Given that most studies using neuroimaging technology to examine the neural correlates of social cognitive processing in patients with mood disorders rely on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we focused our review on this paradigm. We excluded from our evaluation studies that included only behavioural data, observational reports, case studies and studies that were not written in English. ## Results A total of 65 studies41–105 met the appropriate criteria and were included in this review. Appendix 1 (available at cma.ca/jpn) summarizes the results of the reviewed papers. ### Facial emotion processing #### Neural correlates of facial emotion processing Lesion studies of patients with neurologic insult or injury and neuroimaging studies of healthy individuals have revealed a complex neural network involved in the processing of emotional faces. For example, whereas the fusiform gyrus and superior temporal sulcus have been implicated in the perception of facial expressions, the amygdala, anterior insula, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and ventral striatum have been shown to be involved in recognizing emotion and generating emotional reactions in response to provocation stimuli (for a review, see Phillips and colleagues106). The anterior cingulate cortex and prefrontal cortical regions have also been shown to play a prominent role in the regulation of emotion (for reviews, see Ochsner and Gross107 and Phillips and colleagues108). Specifically, recent theoretical models of emotion regulation implicate both top–down and emotion regulation processes in prefrontal cortical regions and the anterior cingulate cortex and bottom–up subcortical and limbic regions involved in emotion appraisal and generation.106,109,110 The DLPFC, which is implicated in working memory and cognitive control, may be involved in the generation and maintenance of emotional reappraisal strategies or ways of transforming/reframing the meaning of an emotional event or stimuli.111–113 The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is thought to be involved in self-monitoring processes,113,114 including evaluating internal states relative to external stimuli.115 The dorsal anterior cingulate cortex has been implicated in conflict error and monitoring,116 including the interference between top–down and bottom–up processes involved in emotion reappraisals and generation.115 These models posit that prefrontal regions, such as the lateral prefrontal cortex, mPFC, OFC and dorsal anterior cingulate, modulate activity in subcortical (e.g., amygdala) and limbic regions.106,113 The amygdala has been implicated in the unconscious and rapid detection of emotionally salient stimuli, including faces,117–119 and a more detailed evaluation of the significance of the emotional stimulus takes place in paralimbic (e.g., thalamus, hippocampus) and cortical regions.120,121 #### Facial emotion processing in patients with major depressive disorder Most studies of social cognition in patients with MDD have examined the perception and recognition of affective stimuli, particularly facial expressions (for a review see Leppänen122). Given the ubiquitous nature of facial expressions, the ability to recognize facial expressions is crucial for intact interpersonal functioning.123 On average, studies examining facial emotion processing in acutely depressed patients have reported a generalized emotion recognition deficit124–133 and impaired recognition of happy facial expressions relative to matched controls.127,129,134–142 Enhanced recognition of sad facial expressions has also been consistently reported in acutely depressed patients.73,143–148 Other studies have reported evidence of a negative bias during facial expression recognition and detection tasks,138,140,149–156 including a tendency to identify neutral faces as sad in patients with moderate to severe depressive symptoms compared with healthy controls.157–159 This bias is accompanied by selective attention to negatively valenced faces depicting sadness134,147,155,160 and anger.161 Overall, these studies point toward a processing bias involving enhanced attention to and recognition of negatively valenced faces during active states of depression that may be accompanied by a tendency to mislabel positively valenced faces as sad and to misjudge (i.e., amplify) the amount of negative emotion conveyed in faces. ### Neuroimaging studies of facial emotion processing in patients with MDD Emotion perception paradigms used in neuroimaging studies of facial emotion processing in patients with mood disorders typically consist of tasks that employ identification and discrimination formats. Identification tasks involve labelling the emotion depicted in individual facial expressions from a fixed set of choices. Although most of these tasks involve recognizing static or still pictures of facial expressions, some studies employ a morphing design that involves dynamic recognition of facial affect by morphing the expression from neutral to beyond the prototypical emotional expression (i.e., 100% emotional expression). Discrimination tasks involve presenting participants with a pair of faces and asking them to judge whether the people in the 2 pictures are expressing the same or a different emotion, or judging the intensity of individual emotional facial expressions. ### Mood state Evidence from fMRI studies suggests that patients with acute MDD differ from healthy controls in the pattern of neural responding arising from exposure to a variety of emotional facial expressions (see Phillips and colleagues162). Specifically, increased activation in the amygdala, ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortices, regions key to the representation of emotion and to the processing of reward, has been reported in response to masked41,49,71,73 and unmasked44,47,51,55,62,70 displays of negatively valenced faces (e.g., expressions of fear, sadness, disgust) in these patients. However, numerous conflicting findings exist.47,62,163,164 They may stem from different emotional processing paradigms and from differences in the clinical status of patients in terms of medication use, illness burden, comorbidity and depression severity. For example, Scheuerecker and colleagues69 recruited a heterogeneous sample of patients experiencing first-episode and recurrent depression and failed to find differences from controls in patterns of neural activation in response to emotional faces; given earlier behavioural and neuroimaging findings that cognitive processing is more vulnerable to disruption in patients with recurrent mood disorders than in first-episode patients (for example, see MacQueen and colleagues,165 Basso and Bornstein,166 Lebowitz and colleagues,167 Milne and colleagues168 and vanGorp and colleagues169), it is possible that this null finding stemmed from the inclusion of first-episode patients in the mood disorders group. Actively depressed patients also show increased neural responses to positive and negative facial expressions in the subgenual portion of the anterior cingulate cortex, a region involved in the physiologic response of emotional processing.45,46,52,63 Increased activity in the subgenual cingulate cortex suggests that individuals with MDD show stronger autonomic emotional responses than controls.63 By contrast, reduced amygdala activation in response to positive stimuli has been demonstrated across a small number of studies.71,99 Suslow and colleagues71 suggest that amygdala hyperactivity to negative stimuli in patients with MDD is associated with negative biases during automatic stages of affective processing,49 including facial affect recognition. Hyporesponsivity of the amygdala to positive stimuli on an automatic processing level is conversely thought to stem from a reduced allocation of cognitive resources for positive stimuli.71,170 A recent report by Lee and colleagues54 also demonstrated reduced activation in the DLPFC in response to the passive presentation of negative facial stimuli among patients with MDD. Similarly, Fales and colleagues52 found that during an emotional distractor task, patients with MDD showed increased amygdala activity and a failure to recruit the DLPFC in response to unattended fearful faces. In contrast, controls demonstrated increased activity in the DLPFC and no activation differences in the amygdala when ignoring fearful faces. These findings are in line with theories of emotion regulation that posit that prefrontal regions, such as the DLPFC, inhibit activity in subcortical (e.g., amygdala) and cortical regions involved in emotional appraisal and emotion generation systems (for example, see Ochsner and Gross109 and Phan and colleagues110). Recent research has also begun to examine the interactions between prefrontal and subcortical regions in patients with MDD. Specifically, during negative facial processing tests (implicit and explicit) consisting of angry and sad facial expressions, the dorsal anterior cingulate and the precuneus, a region implicated in self-related mental representations, show reduced connectivity with the OFC in unmedicated patients with depression.64 Decreased connectivity between the dorsal anterior cingulate and OFC may contribute to dysfunction in the cognitive control of emotional processing (e.g., rewarding value of stimuli) mediated by orbitofrontal regions. The precuneus has been strongly implicated in the mental representation of self and related processes, such as autobiographical recollection and first-person perspective taking; decreased connectivity with the OFC may contribute to the disturbances in self-related processes in patients with mood disorders. Finally, functional connectivity between the OFC and DLPFC was increased in patients compared with controls during negative facial processing; enhanced connectivity between the orbitofrontal region and this key cognitive region, respectively, may give rise to the negative processing bias inherent in the disorder.64 Similarly, another study found that a chronic and recurrent course of illness was significantly associated with reduced functional connectivity between the amygdala and DLPFC while passively viewing angry and sad faces.58 Finally, disruptions in functional coupling between the amygdala and subgenual cingulate, a region also implicated in assessing the salience of emotion and regulating of emotions, have been reported during facial processing tasks.50,55 In contrast, few studies have examined the neural correlates of facial affective processing in euthymic patients with MDD. Preliminary findings by Norbury and colleagues68 demonstrate elevated DLPFC activation in response to fearful faces in euthymic patients compared with controls. Levels of amygdala activation did not differ between patients and matched controls during the presentation of fearful faces. The authors interpreted the increased level of dorsolateral prefrontal activation as a compensatory cortical control mechanism acting to limit emotional dysregulation in limbic regions, including the amygdala, in patients with remitted illness. A recent study by Victor and colleagues,73 however, found that remitted patients with MDD showed exaggerated responses in the amygdala to masked sad faces and a lack of amygdalar response to masked happy faces compared with controls. A possible reason for the discrepant findings in amygdala activation in euthymic patients with MDD may stem from differences in method variance. Specifically, Victor and colleagues73 used an implicit or masked emotional face paradigm, whereas Norbury and colleagues68 used an explicit facial emotion task, which likely engaged the DLPFC to regulate amygdala activity.110,113 The DLPFC has been shown to be activated by explicit emotional tasks relative to tasks that are more implicit in nature.171 Preliminary work has also examined the relation between patterns of neural activation in response to emotional facial expressions and mood state. For example, level of depression severity has been shown to correlate negatively with the extent of activation in the fusiform gyrus,43,47 anterior cingulate cortex48 and amygdala71,74 in response to happy and negative faces.62 It should be noted, however, that an equal number of studies failed to find a significant association between level of depression and neural activity in response to facial emotions.41,49,59,67,69,72 These studies were likely not able to detect a relation between this illness variable and neuronal response because of their limited sample sizes and the inclusion of patients with varying levels of depression. ### Illness burden Few studies have examined the relation between burden of illness variables and neural activity during a facial emotion task. Suslow and colleagues71 reported that amgydala responsiveness to masked sad and happy faces was independent of illness burden variables, such as number of episodes or duration of illness. Similarly, Dannlowski and colleagues49 were unable to detect an association between course of illness variables (duration of illness, number of episodes, total hospital admission time, time since first inpatient treatment and time since first outpatient treatment) and amygdala activity in response to masked displays of angry, sad and happy facial expressions. However, the same study group found that amygdala–DLPFC connectivity was significantly associated with illness severity, indicating that patients with MDD with reduced connectivity between these regions had a more pervasive and severe course of illness.58 ### Medication status Antidepressant therapy may normalize patterns of neuronal responding to affective facial stimuli. For example, a recent study by Fu and colleagues43 examined responding to positive stimuli in patients with MDD compared with matched controls and found reduced activation in subcortical (basal ganglia), limbic (hippocampus) and extrastriate regions among acutely ill patients with MDD; this pattern was attenuated following treatment with fluoxetine. Similarly, Keedwell and colleagues61 found that severely depressed patients showed increased visual cortex responses to sad faces and reduced neural response in these same regions to happy faces in the early stages of antidepressant treatment. Following continued antidepressant therapy and clinical improvement, these patterns were reversed. Moreover, consistent with prior findings demonstrating subgenual cingulate activity as a marker of treatment response (for example, see Mayberg and colleagues172 and Nobler and colleagues173), reductions in subgenual cingulate activity were associated with reductions in depression scores.61 Further analysis of data arising from the study by Keedwell and colleagues61 showed that increased activity in the right visual cortex and subgenual cingulate to sad but not happy facial expressions in the first few weeks of treatment were predictive of a greater clinical recovery.65 In contrast, enhanced responses to happy and sad stimuli in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex were associated with a poor clinical outcome. Similarly, Victor and colleagues73 found that an exaggerated response in the amygdala to masked sad faces and reduced amygdala activity to masked happy faces were reversed following administration of a 4-week treatment regimen of sertraline. These findings indicate that the negative bias toward sad faces improves and a positive bias toward happy faces emerges with antidepressant treatment. Similar to the effects of antidepressant treatment on neural response to emotional facial expressions, administration of erythropoietin, a novel treatment for psychiatric disorders (for example, see Ehrenreich and colleagues174) that exerts neurotrophic and neurorestorative effects, and reduced neural responses in the amygdala and hippocampus to fearful compared with happy faces.66 A recent study has also examined the connectivity of the OFC, a key region in the emotion regulation circuit, to other brain areas in patients with MDD. Lisiecka and colleagues75 found that during a facial emotion identification task, at baseline, an increase in functional coupling between the OFC and motor areas and internally within the right middle OFC was associated with positive response to treatment with anti-depressants (mirtazapine and venlafaxine). In contrast, increased connectivity between the OFC and cerebellum was associated with lack of response to antidepressant treatment. The magnitude of response to antidepressant treatment was also positively correlated with functional coupling between the left OFC and the caudate nuclei and thalamus. This study highlights the utility of functional connectivity patterns of regions implicated in the affect regulation circuit as a marker of treatment response. Overall, these results suggest that conventional antidepressants and novel treatments may dampen hyperactive responses to negative stimuli and enhance the salience of positive stimuli. These findings are also in accordance with the notion that anti-depressants may work by remediating the negative bias in information processing found in patients with MDD.175 ### Psychological treatment status Psychological interventions have also been found to alter patterns of neuronal response to affective stimuli among patients with MDD. Specifically, Fu and colleagues53 found that hyperactivation in the amygdala and hippocampus in response to sad faces appeared to normalize following 16 weeks of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT). Moreover, consistent with prior work showing a significant relation between anterior cingulate activity and clinical response to anti-depressant medication (for example, see Fu and colleagues51), elevated dorsal anterior cingulate activity associated with cognitive control has been shown to be associated with treatment response to CBT.53,57 ### Age Neuroimaging studies conducted in children and adolescents with MDD also show heightened activation in the amygdala56,60,74 and OFC74 in response to emotional facial expressions (for conflicting findings, see Thomas and colleagues42). ### Comorbidity The presence of comorbid disorders, including those on the anxiety spectrum,176,177 may impact patterns of neural activation to affective facial stimuli among patients with MDD. Accordingly, a number of studies reviewed here have excluded patients with comorbid psychiatric diagnoses, including anxiety disorders.43,50,52,53,55,59,64,74,75 Although other studies have explicitly indicated the inclusion of patients with comorbid anxiety disorders,60,62,67,68,71 only Suslow and colleagues71 and Lau and colleagues60 carried out subanalyses to investigate the impact of comorbidity status, finding no significant differences. Two studies42,56 directly compared neural activity in patients with a primary diagnosis of an anxiety disorder and patients with a primary diagnosis of MDD. Thomas and colleagues42 found that during passive viewing of fearful versus neural faces, patients with MDD showed hypoactivation of the right amygdala, whereas hyperactivation of this region was observed in patients with heterogeneous anxiety disorders (generalized anxiety disorder and/or panic disorder). Similar results in the left amygdala were reported by Beesdo and colleagues,56 but only for fearful compared with happy faces and only when viewed passively. ### Section summary Taken together, these findings suggest that individuals with MDD demonstrate altered neural responsiveness to both positively and negatively valenced facial stimuli, regardless of whether they are presented inside or outside of conscious awareness. These results may provide a neural mechanism for the negative bias in facial emotion processing in patients with MDD described in a previous section of this review. Specifically, this negative bias in emotion processing may reflect top–down dysfunction in brain areas, such as the DLPFC and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, implicated in the cognitive control of attention.115,116 The above studies demonstrate hypoactivity and reduced levels of connectivity in these regions in patients with MDD. These findings may be interpreted as deficits in the cognitive control of emotion during the processing of emotionally salient stimuli (faces).49,52,115,116 This negative bias may also stem from deficits in brain areas, such as the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex function (including the subgenual cingulate), implicated in bottom–up emotion processes.106,113 Hyperactivity in the amygdala in response to negative facial stimuli has been demonstrated across numerous studies of patients with MDD, and a small number of studies indicate hypoactivity in this region in response to positive facial stimuli. Increased activity in the subgenual cingulate during baseline rest conditions has been consistently reported (for example, see Drevets and Raichle178 and Mayberg and colleagues179), and an emerging body of research suggests that this region is hyperactive during the processing of emotional faces. Elevated activity and altered connectivity of the amgydala and ventromedial regions, such as the subgenual cingulate, may result in the negative processing bias inherent in the disorder.52 Preliminary evidence suggests that dysfunction in these key emotion processing areas is reversed in the remitted or euthymic state in patients with MDD. These findings suggest that after mood symptoms remit, patients with MDD are able to recruit the DLPFC and dorsal anterior cingulate to suppress activity in the amygdala and related regions and the impact of emotional stimuli, especially negative stimuli. Moreover, preliminary evidence suggests that dysfunction in these key emotion processing regions appears to resolve with antidepressant treatment51,73 and CBT.57 Similar to the behavioural literature, the influence of depressive symptom severity and burden of illness variables on neuronal activity during facial emotion processing in patients with MDD remains to be determined. Finally, few studies have examined the impact of comorbidity on patterns of neural activation, with the studies that do exist showing no differences in patterns of neural activity between patients with and without disease comorbidity. Interestingly, patients with a primary diagnosis of MDD show hyperactivation in the amygdala during active facial emotion tasks and, when compared directly, patients with a primary diagnosis of an anxiety disorder also showed hyperactivation of this region; notably, anxiety samples included a heterogeneous range of illness (e.g., generalized anxiety disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder), rendering the results of these studies indeterminate. #### Facial emotion processing in patients with bipolar disorder Several studies report that patients with bipolar disorder experience difficulty decoding facial emotions on facial affect labelling tasks during periods of mania.180–182 For example, emotion-specific impairments in the identification of sadness,77 disgust181 and fear181 as well as a more generalized deficit in facial emotion recognition have been reported in actively manic patients.24,180 Increased levels of manic symptoms have been shown to correlate significantly with impaired recognition of sad faces.181 In a pattern similar to that observed in patients with unipolar illness, studies examining patients with bipolar disorder in a depressed phase of illness reveal a mood congruent bias in emotion perception tasks, including difficulties labelling happy faces97,102 and a tendency to misinterpret neutral facial stimuli as sad144,183 and happy faces as angry.184 Individuals with bipolar depression are also more accurate than patients with MDD and controls when identifying facial expressions of disgust185 (see Douglas and Porter183 for conflicting findings). Finally, these patients show a reduction in sensitivity to happy facial expressions185–187 that is amplified in the presence of more severe depressive symptoms.186 Hence, whereas mania appears to be associated with a reduction in the ability to recognize negative facial emotions, the presence of depressive symptoms in patients with bipolar disorder appears to result in the overidentification of negative emotion and difficulty recognizing positive affect in faces. ### Neuroimaging studies of facial emotion processing in patients with bipolar disorder Functional neuroimaging studies of facial affect processing in patients with bipolar disorder have revealed mixed findings regarding neural activity in brain regions associated with cognitive and affective processing during facial emotion tasks. ### Mood state In a sample of patients with bipolar disorder with mixed illness states, amygdala activity in response to happy faces was greater in unmedicated patients but lower in medicated patients compared with controls.80 Moreover, compared with controls, rostral anterior cingulate activation was reduced in unmedicated patients with bipolar disorder, whereas medicated patients with bipolar disorder demonstrated similar levels of rostral anterior cingulate activation.80 Killgore and colleagues,88 however, found that a sample of patients with bipolar disorder in varying mood states demonstrated less activation than controls within the putamen, caudate, anterior cingulate gyrus, OFC and superior temporal pole. In addition to the small sample sizes of these studies, heterogeneity within the participant sample in terms of mood state and symptom severity may partially account for these discrepant findings. In depressed patients with bipolar disorder compared with healthy controls, heightened activation in fronto–striato–thalamic regions, including the superior frontal gyrus, ventral frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, cingulate, putamen and thalamus, in response to happy faces has been demonstrated.188 Depressed patients with bipolar disorder have also shown elevated activity in the amygdala102 and attenuated responses in the OFC and DLPFC to neutral and negative (e.g., angry, fearful, sad) faces.30 Evidence of greater left amygdala–OFC connectivity in response to sad faces and reduced bilateral connectivity between these same regions in response to happy faces has also been reported in depressed but not euthymic patients with bipolar disorder.97 These overall findings of elevated levels of neuronal activation in limbic and emotion-related structures and attenuation of activity in areas subserving primarily executive and cognitive processes are similar to those observed in patients with MDD during the performance of similar tasks (see previous sections). Unlike in patients with MDD, however, there is little evidence that this pattern remits with the offset of depressive symptoms. In euthymic patients with bipolar disorder compared with healthy controls, studies have demonstrated increased hippocampal activity82 along with enhanced amygdala activity76 and diminished DLPFC activation when viewing fearful faces76,82,86 (see Hassel and colleagues86 and Robinson and colleagues90 for conflicting findings). A preliminary study by Surguladze and colleagues96 reported that euthymic patients with bipolar disorder also show enhanced activity in the mPFC and amygdala in response to happy facial expressions. It is plausible that the discrepant findings in subcortical activations in both actively ill and euthymic patients may stem from failure to account for comorbid disorders. For example, Hassel and colleagues91 reported significant associations between comorbid symptoms of substance abuse and eating disorders and neural activity in the PFC and subcortical striatal (caudate nucleus, putamen) regions in response to happy and neutral faces. A number of neuroimaging studies have examined facial affect processing in manic patients. Here, a contrasting pattern of reduced amygdala and subgenual cingulate activation in response to sad but not happy facial expressions has been reported in conjunction with a mood-congruent deficit in recognizing sad facial affect.77 Chen and colleagues188 also found that manic patients showed increased activity in the fusiform gyrus in response to sad faces, perhaps leading to enhanced perceptual processing of these stimuli. In that study, a neural response to sad faces was modulated by level of attentional processing. That is, whereas implicit processing of sad faces was associated with enhanced activation in the amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex, lateral temporal cortex and mPFC, explicit processing of sad faces was associated with attenuation of these same regions. Altshuler and colleagues79 reported increased neural response in the amygdala and reduced orbitofrontal cortex response during an emotion discrimination task. Similar to findings in patients with MDD, Foland and colleagues84 also found reduced functional connectivity between the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the amygdala during a facial emotional labelling task in line with reduced inhibitory frontal control over amygdala reactivity. Interestingly, reduced connectivity between the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and amygdala was associated with increased manic symptoms. Moreover, consistent with findings in patients with MDD, patients with bipolar disorder in a variety of mood states showed reduced connectivity between the amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex in response to negatively valenced faces.94 Severity of depressive or manic symptoms may influence neural response to affective facial stimuli in patients with bipolar disorder, although the evidence is inconsistent. For example, patients with more severe depressive illness show significantly enhanced hippocampal activation when viewing sad faces compared to those with milder symptoms of illness.99 Moreover, increased depressive symptom severity was associated with lower activation in the medial frontal gyrus in response to angry faces.98 However, other studies have failed to find significant associations between levels of brain activation and mood state92,94 or symptom severity in patients with bipolar disorder.80,81,86,189 ### Illness burden A limited number of studies have examined the relation between burden of illness variables and neural activity during affective facial processing in patients with bipolar disorder. Recent work has demonstrated significant associations between an earlier illness onset and both amygdala hyperactivity86 and decreased activity within the DLPFC.76 A greater illness burden was associated with reduced functional connectivity between the amygdala and OFC in response to sad faces in a mixed sample of patients in depressed and remitted mood states.97 Foland and colleagues84 also found that reduced connectivity between the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and amygdala was associated with number of previous manic episodes and illness duration in manic patients. It should be noted, however, that Blumberg and colleagues80 did not find any significant associations between level of brain activation and variables such as illness duration or age at onset of illness. Critically, however, patients with bipolar disorder with a longer illness duration and earlier age at illness onset were found to have greater dorsal prefrontal cortex and reduced amygdala activity in response to negatively valenced faces, suggesting an ameliorating effect of a greater illness burden on neural response.91,102 ### Medication status As in patients with MDD, administration of pharmacologic agents may alter the neural circuits underlying the perception and recognition of facial emotion in patients with bipolar disorder. Specifically, 2 recent studies report that patients with bipolar disorder treated with 12 weeks of lamotrigine monotherapy demonstrated “normalized” (increased) activations within the neural circuitry involved in facial affect processing (e.g., medial frontal cortex, precentral and anterior cingulate gyri, middle temporal gyrus) in response to angry85 and sad faces.87 ### Age Finally, neuroimaging studies conducted in adolescents with bipolar disorder similarly showed enhanced activations in subcortical limbic regions and hypoactivation in prefrontal regions in response to emotional faces.30,190,191 These findings have been shown in euthymic youth with bipolar disorder (for example, see Pavuluri and colleagues,83 Rich and colleagues89 and Pavuluri and colleagues93) and heterogeneous samples81,92,95,98 of patients in a variety of mood states. Finally, compared with controls, youth with bipolar disorder demonstrate reduced connectivity between the amygdala and neural regions that are thought to be involved in facial expression processing and emotional stimuli (posterior cingulate/precuneus, fusiform gyrus/parahippocampal gyrus).89 ### Comorbidity Preliminary evidence indicates that comorbid symptoms, and in particular the presence of an anxiety, eating or substance abuse disorder, may alter patterns of neuronal activation during recognition of emotional faces in patients with bipolar disorder, highlighting the need for future studies to consider the presence of comorbidities in their analyses.86,91 Specifically, Hassel and colleagues86 found that, in comparison to patients with bipolar disorder without comorbidities, those with a comorbid diagnosis of any of these disorders showed a trend toward increased activity in the left striatum in response to mildly happy faces during a sex discrimination task. Higher scores on the eating disorder spectrum were also associated with increased activity in the right ventral putamen during identification of intensely happy faces by patients with bipolar disorder.91 Two emotion identification studies89,90 explicitly included Axis I comorbidities with subanalyses indicating no differences in patterns of neural activity (see the section discussing mood state during facial emotion processing in patients with bipolar disorder) after removal of patients with comorbidities. Several studies excluded patients with comorbid conditions.30,80,82,84,85,87,187 ### Section summary These neuroimaging findings support theories proposing abnormally increased activity in subcortical and limbic emotion processing regions (e.g., amygdala) and reduced response in prefrontal cortical emotion regulation regions in patients with bipolar disorder.108 These dysfunctional patterns of neural activity and connectivity are observed in both manic and depressed patients with bipolar disorder (for example, see Foland and colleagues84 and Almeida and colleagues101). However, in contrast to patients with MDD, evidence suggests that in patients with bipolar disorder, alterations in regions implicated in emotion appraisal, generation and regulation may not normalize with remission of mood symptoms.76,82,96 These findings, however, were not consistently observed in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder.86,90 Preliminary research indicates that treatment with a mood stabilizer (lamotrigine) may mitigate dysfunction in neuronal regions involved in the generation and regulation of emotion,85,87 a similar pattern observed after depressive symptoms remit.192 Finally, early evidence of differences in connectivity patterns between the amygdala and OFC in response to displays of facial emotion in patients with bipolar depression may be a potential biomarker that is not present in those with unipolar depression.102 Preliminary evidence indicates that comorbid symptoms, and in particular the presence of anxiety, eating disorder and substance abuse symptoms, may contribute to observed patterns of dysfunction in neural circuits involved in mood regulation in patients with bipolar disorder, highlighting the need for future studies to consider comorbidities in their analyses.86,91 Relatively small sample sizes and variable emotion processing paradigms employed in these studies may also have contributed to discrepant findings observed in subcortical regions, such as the amygdala, across mood states. #### Studies comparing patients with unipolar and bipolar depression A small number of neuroimaging studies comparing individuals with unipolar versus bipolar depression while performing facial affective processing tasks suggest that different pathophysiologic mechanisms may distinguish these 2 types of depression. Almeida and colleagues101 found that recurrent MDD was associated with a top–down negative connectivity between the orbitomedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala in response to positive (happy) faces, suggesting a top–down prefrontal “inhibition” of amygdala activity in response to positive facial expressions. Moreover, in patients with MDD, the greater the medication load, the more positive (less abnormal) the effective connectivity between these 2 regions. In contrast, bipolar depression was associated with a bottom–up disconnectivity between the amygdala and orbitomedial prefrontal cortex, suggesting reduced inhibitory control over amygdala reactivity in patients with this disorder. Patients with bipolar depression have also shown enhanced activation in the amygdala in response to neutral and sad faces compared with controls, patients with remitted bipolar disorder and patients with MDD.102 Finally, Lawrence and colleagues99 also found enhanced levels of activity in limbic and emotion-related structures in patients with bipolar disorder compared with MDD. Specifically, these authors reported that a heterogeneous sample of euthymic and depressed patients with bipolar disorder showed greater activations in subcortical and ventral prefrontal regions in response to positive and negative facial expressions compared with patients with unipolar depression and controls. Overall, these data suggest that patients with bipolar depression may be biased toward negative mood–congruent facial expressions, whereas those with unipolar depression have an attentional bias away from positive expressions.102 #### Influence of facial emotion processing paradigms It should be noted that studies investigating facial emotion comprehension in patients with mood disorders employ diverse methods, such as attention bias,49,52,71,73 emotion identification,30,49,52,55,59,62,64,66–69,71–76,79,82,84,85,87,89–91,93,97,98,101,103,133,185,193–195 sex identification44,45,47,50,51,53,57,64,86,91,92,94,96 and passive viewing paradigms,41,42,54,56,58,60,61,63,66,80,81,83,88,89,95,196 when utilizing fMRI to uncover the neural mechanisms underlying emotion processing. During passive viewing paradigms, patients with bipolar disorder and those with MDD demonstrate hypoactivity in the DLPFC and OFC and hyperactivity in the amygdala and anterior cingulate (subgenual and pregenual), although evidence is less consistent for subcortical regions such as the amygdala and paralimbic regions.41,54,60,63,80,83,95,194 During sex identification tasks, again patients with bipolar disorder and those with MDD demonstrate elevated amygdala responses47,51,92,96 and reduced connectivity between the amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex.50,94 In contrast, preliminary evidence suggests that during sex identification paradigms, patients with MDD show increases in the subgenual anterior cingulate45 and OFC,70 whereas those with bipolar disorder demonstrate less activity in the DLPFC91 and greater activity in the mPFC.96 Facial emotion identification paradigms have converged on several neural regions found to be consistently altered in patients with mood disorders. On average, neural activity is most commonly decreased in the DLPFC30,59,68,76 and increased in the left amygdala,55,74,76,79,98,99 the fusiform gyrus82,93,98 and the inferior frontal gyrus82,90,98 in patients with mood disorders. Interestingly, however, whereas hippocampal and parahippocampal activity is generally increased in patients with bipolar disorder,82,87,98,99 the same regions show decreased activity in those with MDD.72,99 Disturbances in the connectivity between the OFC and the amygdala and cerebellum have also been apparent during identification of emotional faces in patients with MDD75,101 and those with bipolar disorder.97,101 Attention bias paradigms utilizing fMRI have only been carried out in MDD samples, with amygdala hyperactivity emerging as the primary finding. Taken together, these different paradigms tend to converge on a pattern of decreased activation in prefrontal cortical regions associated with emotion regulation and cognitive control (e.g., DLPFC) and increased activation in prefrontal, limbic and subcortical regions associated with affective processing. At present, fMRI studies may not be optimized to detect potential differences in patterns of neural activation between patients with MDD and those with bipolar disorder during social cognitive tasks. Critically, to date, few studies have included separate groups with MDD and with bipolar disorder and have directly compared patterns of neural activation between these groups; most studies included relatively small samples of patients with MDD or bipolar disorder. Moreover, as neuroimaging studies are inherently under temporal constraint, a decreased number of trials may be used to shorten the scan duration, consequently decreasing the statistical power during between- and within-group comparisons. Task complexity is another issue that may contribute to the inconsistency seen during facial emotion recognition. Some fMRI studies69,84 appear to employ emotion recognition tasks that are not difficult or cognitively challenging (for example, see Haldane and colleagues85 and Wilbertz and colleagues163). The goal of these studies was generally to elicit activation from specific brain regions of interest and not necessarily to investigate performance differences in emotion recognition and discrimination.84 Whereas it is beneficial to increase the number of emotions examined for identification to improve ecologic validity, this type of complexity may engage supplementary neural regions (primarily related to ToM) that may be hard to disentangle from neural regions that are predominantly affected in the identification of basic emotions.197 As is generally seen in the neuroimaging literature of emotion identification, although patients with mood disorders may perform comparably to controls on behavioural measures, the neural processing of that information is extensively altered (for example, see Frodl and colleagues64 and Yurgelun-Todd and colleagues76). Another shortfall of the comparison of results within the emotion identification paradigm is the number and types of emotions presented. For example, whereas some studies present only angry and neutral faces, others only present angry and happy faces, making it difficult to yield firm conclusions regarding potential emotion-specific recognition deficits. ### Affective prosody recognition in patients with mood disorders #### Neural correlates of affective prosody recognition Recognition of emotional prosody is thought to draw on multiple processing resources mediated by a complex neural network found primarily in the right superior temporal region.198,199 Wildgruber and colleagues199 recently proposed a neuroanatomical model implicating right-sided primary and association audition areas in the temporal lobe in the perception and extraction of auditory information,200–202 whereas posterior regions of the right superior temporal sulcus are thought to be involved in representing the meaning of the acoustic sequences.196,201 In contrast, evaluative aspects of affective prosody identification are believed to be mediated by the bilateral inferior frontal cortex;200,202,203 this has been reviewed by Wildgruber and colleagues199 and Adolphs and colleagues.204 #### Affective prosody recognition in patients with bipolar disorder Preliminary evidence indicates that individuals with mood disorders experience difficulty in the recognition of affective prosody. In a pattern similar to the results reported for identification of facial emotion, a bias toward interpreting neutral prosodic emotions (i.e., surprise) as negative,152,205 and difficulty identifying both positively valenced206,207 and negatively valenced206,207 emotional tones (for contradictory findings, see Uekermann and colleagues208) have been reported in homogeneous samples of acutely depressed patients. Similar findings of impaired recognition of positively206 and of negatively valenced206,209 tones have been reported in both actively ill and euthymic209 patients with bipolar disorder (for contradictory findings, see Harmer175). ### Neuroimaging studies of affective prosody recognition in patients with bipolar disorder Despite these preliminary behavioural findings, the neural correlates of altered affective prosody recognition in patients with mood disorders remain relatively unexplored. One preliminary study by Mitchell and colleagues78 compared the neural responses of controls and patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder to recorded scenarios presented in happy, sad and neutral intonations. Patients were scanned while passively listening to affective prosody stimuli and while actively attending to the emotional intonation of each phrase. During the passive listening task, compared with controls, the bipolar group demonstrated less activation in the amygdala, uncus, bilateral superior temporal gyrus and right inferior frontal gyrus in response to pure emotional prosody and greater activation of the left superior temporal gyrus in response to unfiltered emotional prosody. The lack of activation found in the right-sided prefrontal and temporal areas implicated in affective prosody recognition (see the section discussing neural correlates of affective processing recognition) suggests a reduced neural capacity to process this type of emotional stimuli.78 #### Mood state, illness burden, medication status, age The influence of mood state, illness burden, medication status and age on neural response during affective prosodic processing remains to be investigated. ### Theory of mind in patients with mood disorders #### Neural correlates of ToM Theory of mind refers to the ability to infer the mental states of other individuals, including their beliefs, desires and intentions, to explain or predict human behaviour.210 Recent theoretical models propose that ToM draws on both cognitive (e.g., understanding another’s perspective) and affective (e.g., emotional response to feeling states of others) processing resources.211,212 Neuroimaging and behavioural studies of ToM implicate a core network of neural regions that serve diverse functions, and include cognitive (e.g., DLPFC19,213,214), affective (e.g., mPFC,215–219 anterior paracingulate cortex220,221) and memory systems (e.g., posterior cingulate, temporal poles;220 for a review, see McKinnon and colleagues222). Moreover, neuroimaging evidence also implicates the posterior superior temporal sulcus, which is involved in biological motion perception,223,224 including socially relevant directional cues such as the eye gaze of others,225,226 and the adjacent temporoparietal junction, which is involved in the attribution of beliefs to others,227 as critical for ToM ability.7,228,229 #### Theory of mind ability in patients with mood disorders Theory of mind ability in patients with mood disorders remains an underexplored area of research230 despite recent findings that impaired ToM ability may be associated with poor clinical and functional outcome in these patients.231 A small number of studies conducted in actively depressed patients with MDD suggest that these patients show impairments on ToM tasks that involve decoding mental states from available information, such as facial expressions, tone of voice, gestures and reasoning232–234 (for contradictory findings, see Wolkenstein and colleagues235) and tasks that involve reasoning about mental states by combining contextual information and prior knowledge about an individual or situation to understand behaviour236 (for contradictory results in patients with varying levels of symptom severity, see Wilbertz and colleagues163). Similar findings have been reported for patients with active and subsyndromal symptoms of bipolar disorder.164,237 Importantly, these deficits appear to persist into the euthymic state in patients with MDD238 and with bipolar disorder,10,239,240 particularly for those ToM tasks that place high demands on cognitive processing resources (e.g., working memory, executive functioning; for an extended discussion of this issue, see McKinnon and colleagues237). #### Neuroimaging findings: ToM ##### Mood state To date, studies examining the neural correlates of ToM processing in patients with mood disorders have been confined to investigations of patients with bipolar disorder; 2 preliminary investigations have examined euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Malhi and colleagues82 instructed patients with bipolar disorder and matched controls to observe computer-animated ToM stimuli depicting complex mental states, such as bluffing, persuading, surprising and mocking. Patients performed more poorly than controls on this task. These authors also found that, compared with controls, patients with bipolar disorder demonstrated a diminished pattern of activation in the supramarginal, angular and middle temporal gyri. These regions have been shown previously to have strong functional connectivity with the temporoparietal junction and superior temporal sulcus,241 regions thought to be crucial to ToM. Moreover, the patient sample showed a reduced neural response in the insula and bilateral inferior frontal gyri during viewing of these stimuli. These regions are strongly implicated in the mirror neuron system involved in decoding the actions and intentions of others.242 In a separate study, Kim and colleagues105 examined the performance of euthymic patients with bipolar disorder on a social cognitive task that involved inferring the reason for the expressed emotion of a virtual human (avatar) based on emotional cues (facial expression of avatar and verbal description of experience). Similar to the findings of Malhi and colleagues,104 euthymic patients with bipolar disorder showed hypoactivation in mirror neuron system regions, such as the right inferior frontal gyrus, right insula and right premotor cortex, during happy and angry emotional conditions.105 Moreover, although the bipolar disorder group performed comparably to controls on the social cognitive task, they showed significantly delayed response times during the emotional conditions. ##### Medication status Kim and colleagues105 found that neither the number of medications nor medication dosage influenced neuronal activation during ToM performance. ##### Illness burden, age The effect of illness burden and age on neuronal activation during ToM performance in patients with mood disorders remains an underexplored area of research. ## Conclusion Alterations in social cognitive functioning in patients with mood disorders are likely related to alterations in patterns of neuronal activation observed during performance of these tasks. Functional neuroimaging studies of facial emotion processing in patients with MDD and those with bipolar disorder implicate altered interactions between subcortical, limbic and ventral prefrontal regions associated with emotion identification and production, and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal regions associated with emotion regulation and higher cognitive functions, which may vary with the emotional valence (i.e., positive, negative) of affective stimuli. Taken together, these results point toward an overall lack of inhibition by higher-order cognitive structures on limbic and emotion-related structures during social cognitive processing in patients with mood disorders that is consistent across different social cognitive tasks, including recognition of emotion in faces and prosody, as well as ToM. Critically, our review of the primary body of literature in this area concerning facial emotion processing suggests that whereas this pattern is likely to return to levels of activation seen in healthy controls following symptom remission in patients with MDD, patients with bipolar disorder do not show this normalization of function despite recovery (for exceptions following treatment with a mood stabilizer, see Hassel and colleagues86 and Robinson and colleagues90). Interestingly, consistent with the results of mood provocation studies (for a review, see Ressler and Mayberg243), very preliminary evidence indicates that psychotropic medications and CBT may exert different effects on the neural networks involved in affective recognition. Specifically, whereas treatment with psychotropic medication appears to attenuate levels of activation in prefrontal and subcortical regions involved in affective processing, CBT appears to increase levels of neural activation in regions associated with emotion regulation and higher-order cognitive processing. It will be critical for future studies to examine the effect of different medication classes and psychologic treatments on social cognitive function in patients with mood disorders. A number of other key methodologic and demographic variables may further moderate patterns of neuronal activation during social cognitive processing in patients with mood disorders. Specifically, patients with a lower burden of illness (i.e., fewer disease episodes or shorter duration of illness) may be less likely to show alteration in the circuitry underlying social cognitive processing in the early stages of illness. Findings concerning the impact of symptom severity among actively ill patients have been less consistent and, on average, have failed to reveal a relation between severity of active illness and patterns of neural activation; these findings may be challenged with the inclusion of larger samples of patients with groups ranging in symptom severity. Finally, the inclusion of heterogeneous samples of patients with comorbid diagnoses, particularly anxiety disorders, may limit the interpretability of findings. Future studies may seek to directly compare patients with mood disorders and a single anxiety disorder, such as PTSD, which is well known to impact the neural circuitry involved in cognitive and affective processing. Finally, social cognitive tasks that place high demands on cognitive processing resources (e.g., executive functioning, working memory) may engage higher-order cognitive regions to a greater extent than those social cognitive tasks having lower-order processing demands; these tasks may prove particularly challenging to patients with a more severe burden of illness and elevated level of symptom severity. A nascent literature suggests that youth with MDD and with bipolar disorder show impaired performance on tests of facial emotion processing, a finding underscored by studies demonstrating early alterations in the neuronal circuitry underlying social cognitive processing in these samples. Social reasoning abilities, such as emotion comprehension, ToM, and empathy, undergo an extended development from early childhood to adolescence.7,244,245 Adolescence, a period of time characterized by marked changes in social relationships with peers and family,246,247 is also associated with increased vulnerability to depression and bipolar disorder.248–250 It is possible that any alterations in social cognitive processes and their neuronal underpinnings during this period of development may contribute to the early onset of mood disorders. Alternatively, the development of psychiatric morbidity during adolescence could alter or delay the development of social cognitive abilities and the development of neuronal circuits mediating these processes. A better understanding of social cognition in patients with mood disorders and of factors that may influence social cognitive functioning could improve early intervention efforts aimed at reducing the peak morbidity and mortality observed in youth. The study of social cognitive processing in patients with mood disorders is still in an early stage of discovery, where many outstanding questions remain concerning: for example, the influence of medications, course of illness variables and cognitive status on task performance and patterns of neuronal activation. Given the heterogeneous profile of these disorders in terms of symptom severity, chronicity, recurrence, comorbidities and treatment status, it will be important for future studies to incorporate larger sample sizes to take into account the influence of these variables on social cognitive processing. In particular, small sample sizes in fMRI paradigms may have constrained the ability to detect performance differences among groups (for example, see Canli and colleagues,44 Chen and colleagues,48 Schaefer and colleagues,100 Gotlib and colleagues,134 Gilboa-Schechtman and colleagues156 and Bediou and colleagues251). Longitudinal studies, for example, will be necessary to determine whether social cognitive deficits are trait markers of vulnerability for patients with mood disorders. Investigations recruiting individuals at risk for mood disorders will also address this issue. Future studies may also determine whether known alterations in neuronal structure (e.g., reduced hippocampal volume) in patients with MDD and with bipolar disorder are associated with altered patterns of social cognitive performance across multiple social cognitive domains, including emotion recognition, ToM, empathy and moral reasoning. An enhanced understanding of social cognition in patients with mood disorders may contribute to new advances in the treatment of these illnesses. ## Acknowledgements This article was supported by an Ontario Mental Health Foundation (OMHF) studentship to A.M. Cusi and grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, OMHF and National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression to M.C. McKinnon. ## Footnotes * **Contributors:** G.M. MacQueen and M.C. McKinnon designed the review. A.M. Cusi, A. Nazarov, K. Holshausen and M.C. McKinnon acquired the data. A.M. Cusi and M.C. McKinnon analyzed the data. A.M. Cusi, A. Nazarov, K. Holshausen and M.C. McKinnon wrote the article. A.M. Cusi, G.M. MacQueen and M.C. McKinnon reviewed the article. All authors approved its publication. * **Competing interests:** None declared for A.M. Cusi, A. Nazarov, K. Holshausen and M.C. McKinnon. G.M. MacQueen declares that her institute has received grant support from AstraZeneca and that she has received speaker fees from AstraZeneca, BMS, Janssen, Lilly and Pfizer and payment for development of educational presentations from the Canadian Psychiatric Association. * Received December 15, 2010. * Revision received July 2, 2011. * Revision received September 2, 2011. * Accepted September 8, 2011. ## References 1. Begley CE, Annegers JF, Swann AC, et al.The lifetime cost of bipolar disorder in the US: an estimate for new cases in 1998.Pharmacoeconomics 2001;19:483–95. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.2165/00019053-200119050-00004&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11465308&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000168959900004&link_type=ISI) 2. Kessler RC, Akiskal HS, Ames M, et al.Prevalence and effects of mood disorders on work performance in a nationally representative sample of U.S. workers.Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:1561–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.163.9.1561&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16946181&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000240205100016&link_type=ISI) 3. Romera I, Perez V, Menchon JM, et al.Social and occupational functioning impairment in patients in partial versus complete remission of a major depressive disorder episode. A six-month prospective epidemiological study.Eur Psychiatry 2010;25:58–65. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19553092&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000274057100010&link_type=ISI) 4. Wells KB, Stewart A, Hays RD, et al.The functioning and well-being of depressed patients. Results from the Medical Outcomes Study.JAMA 1989;262:914–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/jama.1989.03430070062031&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=2754791&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1989AK28900025&link_type=ISI) 5. Adolphs R.The neurobiology of social cognition.Curr Opin Neurobiol 2001;11:231–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00202-6&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11301245&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000168505500014&link_type=ISI) 6. Brothers L.The social brain: a project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain.Concepts Neurosci 1990;1:27–51. 7. Frith U, Frith CD.Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing.Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2003;358:459–73. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1098/rstb.2002.1218&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12689373&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000181940000004&link_type=ISI) 8. Phillips AT, Wellman HM, Spelke ES.Infants’ ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action.Cognition 2002;85:53–78. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00073-2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12086713&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000176966900003&link_type=ISI) 9. Bora E, Gokcen S, Veznedaroglu B.Empathic abilities in people with schizophrenia.Psychiatry Res 2008;160:23–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.psychres.2007.05.017&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18514324&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000257645800004&link_type=ISI) 10. Bora E, Vahip S, Gonul AS, et al.Evidence for theory of mind deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder.Acta Psychiatr Scand 2005;112:110–6. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00570.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15992392&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000230292000005&link_type=ISI) 11. Brüne M.Emotion recognition, ‘theory of mind,’ and social behavior in schizophrenia.Psychiatry Res 2005;133:135–47. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.psychres.2004.10.007&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15740990&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000227815100004&link_type=ISI) 12. Buitelaar JK, van der Wees M.Are deficits in the decoding of affective cues and in mentalizing abilities independent?.J Autism Dev Disord 1997;27:539–56. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1023/A:1025878026569&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9403371&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1997YJ08100004&link_type=ISI) 13. Carr MB, Lutjemeier JA.The relation of facial affect recognition and empathy to delinquency in youth offenders.Adolescence 2005;40:601–19. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16268136&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000232309800011&link_type=ISI) 14. Decety J, Jackson PL.The functional architecture of human empathy.Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 2004;3:71–100. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/1534582304267187&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15537986&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 15. Henry JD, Phillips LH, Crawford JR, et al.Theory of mind following traumatic brain injury: the role of emotion recognition and executive dysfunction.Neuropsychologia 2006;44:1623–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.020&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16643969&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000239403200001&link_type=ISI) 16. Adolphs R.The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge.Annu Rev Psychol 2009;60:693–716. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18771388&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000262615800028&link_type=ISI) 17. Ongür D, Price JL.The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans.Cereb Cortex 2000;10:206–19. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/cercor/10.3.206&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10731217&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000085865600002&link_type=ISI) 18. Rankin KP, Kramer JH, Miller BL.Patterns of cognitive and emotional empathy in frontotemporal lobar degeneration.Cogn Behav Neurol 2005;18:28–36. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/01.wnn.0000152225.05377.ab&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15761274&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000229236600004&link_type=ISI) 19. Stone VE, Baron-Cohen S, Knight RT.Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind.J Cogn Neurosci 1998;10:640–56. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1162/089892998562942&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9802997&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000077593600009&link_type=ISI) 20. Altshuler LL, Ventura J, van Gorp WG, et al.Neurocognitive function in clinically stable men with bipolar I disorder or schizophrenia and normal control subjects.Biol Psychiatry 2004;56:560–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.002&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15476685&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000224538800005&link_type=ISI) 21. Decety J, Lamm C.The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: how low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition.Neuroscientist 2007;13:580–93. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/1073858407304654&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17911216&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000251065900010&link_type=ISI) 22. Olson IR, Plotzker A, Ezzyat Y.The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing.Brain 2007;130Pt 71718–31. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/brain/awm052&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17392317&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000248151300003&link_type=ISI) 23. Price JL, Drevets WC.Neurocircuitry of mood disorders.Neuropsychopharmacology 2010;35:192–216. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/npp.2009.104&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19693001&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000272784600011&link_type=ISI) 24. Bozikas VP, Tonia T, Fokas K, et al.Impaired emotion processing in remitted patients with bipolar disorder.J Affect Disord 2006;91:53–6. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2005.11.013&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16413611&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000236154400008&link_type=ISI) 25. Mur M, Portella MJ, Martinez-Arán A, et al.Persistent neuropsychological deficit in euthymic bipolar patients: executive function as a core deficit.J Clin Psychiatry 2007;68:1078–86. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.4088/JCP.v68n0715&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17685745&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000248494300015&link_type=ISI) 26. Hasselbalch BJ, Knorr U, Kessing LV.Cognitive impairment in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder: a systematic review.J Affect Disord 2011;134:20–31. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.011&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21163534&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 27. Preiss M, Kucerova H, Lukavsky J, et al.Cognitive deficits in the euthymic phase of unipolar depression.Psychiatry Res 2009;169:235–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.042&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19765829&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 28. Bourke C, Douglas K, Porter R.Processing of facial emotion expression in major depression: a review.Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2010;44:681–96. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.3109/00048674.2010.496359&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20636189&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 29. King MJ, MacDougall AG, Ferris SM, et al.A review of factors that moderate autobiographical memory performance in patients with major depressive disorder.J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2010;32:1122–44. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20544462&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 30. Altshuler L, Bookheimer S, Townsend J, et al.Regional brain changes in bipolar I depression: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Bipolar Disord 2008;10:708–17. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00617.x&link_type=DOI) 31. Depp CA, Mausbach BT, Harvey PD, et al.Social competence and observer-rated social functioning in bipolar disorder.Bipolar Disord 2010;12:843–50. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00880.x&link_type=DOI) 32. Elgie R, Morselli PL.Social functioning in bipolar patients: the perception and perspective of patients, relatives and advocacy organizations — a review.Bipolar Disord 2007;9:144–57. 33. Hirschfeld RM, Dunner DL, Keitner G, et al.Does psychosocial functioning improve independent of depressive symptoms? A comparison of nefazodone, psychotherapy, and their combination.Biol Psychiatry 2002;51:123–33. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01291-4&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11822991&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000173570900001&link_type=ISI) 34. Miller IW, Keitner GI, Schatzberg AF, et al.The treatment of chronic depression, part 3: psychosocial functioning before and after treatment with sertraline or imipramine.J Clin Psychiatry 1998;59:608–19. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.4088/JCP.v59n1108&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9862607&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000077383000008&link_type=ISI) 35. Judd LL, Akiskal HS, Schettler PJ, et al.A prospective investigation of the natural history of the long-term weekly symptomatic status of bipolar II disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60:261–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.60.3.261&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12622659&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000181572200006&link_type=ISI) 36. Judd LL, Akiskal HS, Schettler PJ, et al.The long-term natural history of the weekly symptomatic status of bipolar I disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;59:530–7. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.59.6.530&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12044195&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000176055400007&link_type=ISI) 37. Ghaemi SN, Sachs GS, Chiou AM, et al.Is bipolar disorder still underdiagnosed? Are antidepressants overutilized?.J Affect Disord 1999;52:135–44. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0165-0327(98)00076-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10357026&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000080305800015&link_type=ISI) 38. Hirschfeld RM, Lewis L, Vornik LA.Perceptions and impact of bipolar disorder: How far have we really come? Results of the national depressive and manic-depressive association 2000 survey of individuals with bipolar disorder.J Clin Psychiatry 2003;64:161–74. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.4088/JCP.v64n0209&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12633125&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000181289900009&link_type=ISI) 39. Awad AG, Rajagopalan K, Bolge SC, et al.Quality of life among bipolar disorder patients misdiagnosed with major depressive disorder. Prim Care Companion.J Clin Psychiatry 2007;9:195–202. 40. Ghaemi SN, Boiman EE, Goodwin FK.Diagnosing bipolar disorder and the effect of antidepressants: a naturalistic study.J Clin Psychiatry 2000;61:804–8quiz 9 [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.4088/JCP.v61n1013&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11078046&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000165173000023&link_type=ISI) 41. Sheline YI, Barch DM, Donnelly JM, et al.Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study.Biol Psychiatry 2001;50:651–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01263-X&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11704071&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000172035100001&link_type=ISI) 42. Thomas KM, Drevets WC, Dahl RE, et al.Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58:1057–63. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1057&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11695953&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000172056100009&link_type=ISI) 43. Fu CH, Williams SC, Brammer MJ, et al.Neural responses to happy facial expressions in major depression following antidepressant treatment.Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:599–607. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.164.4.599&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17403973&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000245402600014&link_type=ISI) 44. Canli T, Cooney RE, Goldin P, et al.Amygdala reactivity to emotional faces predicts improvement in major depression.Neuroreport 2005;16:1267–70. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/01.wnr.0000174407.09515.cc&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16056122&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000231492600003&link_type=ISI) 45. Gotlib IH, Sivers H, Gabrieli JD, et al.Subgenual anterior cingulate activation to valenced emotional stimuli in major depression.Neuroreport 2005;16:1731–4. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/01.wnr.0000183901.70030.82&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16237317&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000233277700002&link_type=ISI) 46. Keedwell PA, Andrew C, Williams SC, et al.A double dissociation of ventromedial prefrontal cortical responses to sad and happy stimuli in depressed and healthy individuals.Biol Psychiatry 2005;58:495–503. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.035&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15993859&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000232296000010&link_type=ISI) 47. Surguladze S, Brammer MJ, Keedwell P, et al.A differential pattern of neural response toward sad versus happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:201–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.028&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15691520&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000226886400001&link_type=ISI) 48. Chen CH, Ridler K, Suckling J, et al.Brain imaging correlates of depressive symptom severity and predictors of symptom improvement after antidepressant treatment.Biol Psychiatry 2007;62:407–14. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.018&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17217921&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000249042800006&link_type=ISI) 49. Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, et al.Amygdala reactivity to masked negative faces is associated with automatic judgmental bias in major depression: a 3 T fMRI study.J Psychiatry Neurosci 2007;32:423–9. 50. Chen CH, Suckling J, Ooi C, et al.Functional coupling of the amygdala in depressed patients treated with antidepressant medication.Neuropsychopharmacology 2008;33:1909–18. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/sj.npp.1301593&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17987064&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000256691900013&link_type=ISI) 51. Fu CH, Williams SC, Cleare AJ, et al.Attenuation of the neural response to sad faces in major depression by antidepressant treatment: a prospective, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61:877–89. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.877&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15351766&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000223726200003&link_type=ISI) 52. Fales CL, Barch DM, Rundle MM, et al.Altered emotional interference processing in affective and cognitive-control brain circuitry in major depression.Biol Psychiatry 2008;63:377–84. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.012&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17719567&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000252766300006&link_type=ISI) 53. Fu CH, Williams SC, Cleare AJ, et al.Neural responses to sad facial expressions in major depression following cognitive behavioral therapy.Biol Psychiatry 2008;64:505–12. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.033&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18550030&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000258892000010&link_type=ISI) 54. Lee BT, Seok JH, Lee BC, et al.Neural correlates of affective processing in response to sad and angry facial stimuli in patients with major depressive disorder.Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2008;32:778–85. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.12.009&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18207298&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 55. Matthews SC, Strigo IA, Simmons AN, et al.Decreased functional coupling of the amygdala and supragenual cingulate is related to increased depression in unmedicated individuals with current major depressive disorder.J Affect Disord 2008;111:13–20. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.022&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18603301&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 56. Beesdo K, Lau JY, Guyer AE, et al.Common and distinct amygdala-function perturbations in depressed vs anxious adolescents.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66:275–85. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.545&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19255377&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000263765600006&link_type=ISI) 57. Costafreda SG, Khanna A, Mourao-Miranda J, et al.Neural correlates of sad faces predict clinical remission to cognitive behavioural therapy in depression.Neuroreport 2009;20:637–41. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283294159&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19339907&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000265868800001&link_type=ISI) 58. Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Konrad C, et al.Reduced amygdala-prefrontal coupling in major depression: association with MAOA genotype and illness severity.Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009;12:11–22. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S1461145708008973&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18544183&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 59. Fales CL, Barch DM, Rundle MM, et al.Antidepressant treatment normalizes hypoactivity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during emotional interference processing in major depression.J Affect Disord 2009;112:206–11. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2008.04.027&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18559283&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000261840100024&link_type=ISI) 60. Lau JY, Goldman D, Buzas B, et al.Amygdala function and 5-HTT gene variants in adolescent anxiety and major depressive disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2009;65:349–55. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.037&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18950748&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000263249900013&link_type=ISI) 61. Keedwell P, Drapier D, Surguladze S, et al.Neural markers of symptomatic improvement during antidepressant therapy in severe depression: subgenual cingulate and visual cortical responses to sad, but not happy, facial stimuli are correlated with changes in symptom score.J Psychopharmacol 2009;23:775–88. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/0269881108093589&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18635699&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000269027100004&link_type=ISI) 62. Peluso MA, Glahn DC, Matsuo K, et al.Amygdala hyperactivation in untreated depressed individuals.Psychiatry Res 2009;173:158–61. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.03.006&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19545982&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000269113000013&link_type=ISI) 63. Baeken C, Van Schuerbeek P, De Raedt R, et al.Reduced left subgenual anterior cingulate cortical activity during withdrawal-related emotions in melancholic depressed female patients.J Affect Disord 2010;127:326–31. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20547425&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 64. Frodl T, Bokde AL, Scheuerecker J, et al.Functional connectivity bias of the orbitofrontal cortex in drug-free patients with major depression.Biol Psychiatry 2010;67:161–7. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.022&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19811772&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000273201800011&link_type=ISI) 65. Keedwell PA, Drapier D, Surguladze S, et al.Subgenual cingulate and visual cortex responses to sad faces predict clinical outcome during antidepressant treatment for depression.J Affect Disord 2010;120:120–5. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.031&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19539998&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 66. Miskowiak KW, Favaron E, Hafizi S, et al.Erythropoietin modulates neural and cognitive processing of emotional information in biomarker models of antidepressant drug action in depressed patients.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;210:419–28. 67. Moses-Kolko EL, Perlman SB, Wisner KL, et al.Abnormally reduced dorsomedial prefrontal cortical activity and effective connectivity with amygdala in response to negative emotional faces in postpartum depression.Am J Psychiatry 2010;167:1373–80. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09081235&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20843875&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000283669500014&link_type=ISI) 68. Norbury R, Selvaraj S, Taylor MJ, et al.Increased neural response to fear in patients recovered from depression: a 3T functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Psychol Med 2010;40:425–32. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0033291709990596&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19627640&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 69. Scheuerecker J, Meisenzahl EM, Koutsouleris N, et al.Orbitofrontal volume reductions during emotion recognition in patients with major depression.J Psychiatry Neurosci 2010;35:311–20. 70. Surguladze SA, El-Hage W, Dalgleish T, et al.Depression is associated with increased sensitivity to signals of disgust: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.J Psychiatr Res 2010;44:894–902. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.02.010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20307892&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 71. Suslow T, Konrad C, Kugel H, et al.Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in major depression.Biol Psychiatry 2010;67:155–60. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.023&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19748075&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000273201800010&link_type=ISI) 72. Townsend JD, Eberhart NK, Bookheimer SY, et al.fMRI activation in the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex in unmedicated subjects with major depressive disorder.Psychiatry Res 2010;183:209–17. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.06.001&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20708906&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000282142800005&link_type=ISI) 73. Victor TA, Furey ML, Fromm SJ, et al.Relationship between amygdala responses to masked faces and mood state and treatment in major depressive disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010;67:1128–38. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.144&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21041614&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000283737500009&link_type=ISI) 74. Yang TT, Simmons AN, Matthews SC, et al.Adolescents with major depression demonstrate increased amygdala activation.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010;49:42–51. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20215925&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 75. Lisiecka D, Meisenzahl E, Scheuerecker J, et al.Neural correlates of treatment outcome in major depression.Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2011;14:521–34. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S1461145710001513&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21205435&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 76. Yurgelun-Todd DA, Gruber SA, Kanayama G, et al.fMRI during affect discrimination in bipolar affective disorder.Bipolar Disord 2000;2:237–48. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1034/j.1399-5618.2000.20304.x&link_type=DOI) 77. Lennox BR, Jacob R, Calder AJ, et al.Behavioural and neurocognitive responses to sad facial affect are attenuated in patients with mania.Psychol Med 2004;34:795–802. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0033291704002557&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15500300&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000223139500004&link_type=ISI) 78. Mitchell RL, Elliott R, Barry M, et al.Neural response to emotional prosody in schizophrenia and in bipolar affective disorder.Br J Psychiatry 2004;184:223–30. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6ImJqcHJjcHN5Y2giO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6OToiMTg0LzMvMjIzIjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTg6Ii9qcG4vMzcvMy8xNTQuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 79. Altshuler L, Bookheimer S, Proenza MA, et al.Increased amygdala activation during mania: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Am J Psychiatry 2005;162:1211–3. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1211&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15930074&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000229504300028&link_type=ISI) 80. Blumberg HP, Donegan NH, Sanislow CA, et al.Preliminary evidence for medication effects on functional abnormalities in the amygdala and anterior cingulate in bipolar disorder.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:308–13. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s00213-005-0156-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16249909&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 81. Rich BA, Vinton DT, Roberson-Nay R, et al.Limbic hyperactivation during processing of neutral facial expressions in children with bipolar disorder.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103:8900–5. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoicG5hcyI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMToiMTAzLzIzLzg5MDAiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoxODoiL2pwbi8zNy8zLzE1NC5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 82. Malhi GS, Lagopoulos J, Sachdev PS, et al.Is a lack of disgust something to fear? A functional magnetic resonance imaging facial emotion recognition study in euthymic bipolar disorder patients.Bipolar Disord 2007;9:345–57. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00485.x&link_type=DOI) 83. Pavuluri MN, O’Connor MM, Harral E, et al.Affective neural circuitry during facial emotion processing in pediatric bipolar disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2007;62:158–67. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.011&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17097071&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000247961700009&link_type=ISI) 84. Foland LC, Altshuler L, Bookheimer S, et al.Evidence for deficient modulation of amygdala response by prefrontal cortex in bipolar mania.Psychiatry Res 2008;162:27–37. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.04.007&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18063349&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000253442900003&link_type=ISI) 85. Haldane M, Jogia J, Cobb A, et al.Changes in brain activation during working memory and facial recognition tasks in patients with bipolar disorder with lamotrigine monotherapy.Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2008;18:48–54. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.euroneuro.2007.05.009&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17618089&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 86. Hassel S, Almeida JR, Kerr N, et al.Elevated striatal and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity in response to emotional stimuli in euthymic bipolar disorder: no associations with psychotropic medication load.Bipolar Disord 2008;10:916–27. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1399-5618.2008.00641.x&link_type=DOI) 87. Jogia J, Haldane M, Cobb A, et al.Pilot investigation of the changes in cortical activation during facial affect recognition with lamotrigine monotherapy in bipolar disorder.Br J Psychiatry 2008;192:197–201. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6ImJqcHJjcHN5Y2giO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6OToiMTkyLzMvMTk3IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTg6Ii9qcG4vMzcvMy8xNTQuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 88. Killgore WD, Gruber SA, Yurgelun-Todd DA.Abnormal cortico-striatal activity during fear perception in bipolar disorder.Neuroreport 2008;19:1523–7. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/WNR.0b013e328310af58&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18797310&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000259728400016&link_type=ISI) 89. Rich BA, Fromm SJ, Berghorst LH, et al.Neural connectivity in children with bipolar disorder: impairment in the face emotion processing circuit.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008;49:88–96. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01819.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18181882&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000253720700009&link_type=ISI) 90. Robinson JL, Monkul ES, Tordesillas-Gutierrez D, et al.Frontolimbic circuitry in euthymic bipolar disorder: evidence for prefrontal hyperactivation.Psychiatry Res 2008;164:106–13. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.12.004&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18930635&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000261551600002&link_type=ISI) 91. Hassel S, Almeida JR, Frank E, et al.Prefrontal cortical and striatal activity to happy and fear faces in bipolar disorder is associated with comorbid substance abuse and eating disorder.J Affect Disord 2009;118:19–27. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.021&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19243839&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 92. Kalmar JH, Wang F, Chepenik LG, et al.Relation between amygdala structure and function in adolescents with bipolar disorder.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009;48:636–42. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819f6fbc&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19454919&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000266298100008&link_type=ISI) 93. Pavuluri MN, Passarotti AM, Harral EM, et al.An fMRI study of the neural correlates of incidental versus directed emotion processing in pediatric bipolar disorder.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009;48:308–19. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181948fc7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19242292&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000263742100010&link_type=ISI) 94. Wang F, Kalmar JH, He Y, et al.Functional and structural connectivity between the perigenual anterior cingulate and amygdala in bipolar disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2009;66:516–21. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.023&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19427632&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000269330900015&link_type=ISI) 95. Brotman MA, Rich BA, Guyer AE, et al.Amygdala activation during emotion processing of neutral faces in children with severe mood dysregulation versus ADHD or bipolar disorder.Am J Psychiatry 2010;167:61–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09010043&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19917597&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000273575900011&link_type=ISI) 96. Surguladze SA, Marshall N, Schulze K, et al.Exaggerated neural response to emotional faces in patients with bipolar disorder and their first-degree relatives.Neuroimage 2010;53:58–64. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.069&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20595014&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000280818900007&link_type=ISI) 97. Versace A, Thompson WK, Zhou D, et al.Abnormal left and right amygdala-orbitofrontal cortical functional connectivity to emotional faces: state versus trait vulnerability markers of depression in bipolar disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2010;67:422–31. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.025&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20159144&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000275100300005&link_type=ISI) 98. Passarotti AM, Sweeney JA, Pavuluri MN.Emotion processing influences working memory circuits in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010;49:1064–80. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jaac.2010.07.009&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20855051&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 99. Lawrence NS, Williams AM, Surguladze S, et al.Subcortical and ventral prefrontal cortical neural responses to facial expressions distinguish patients with bipolar disorder and major depression.Biol Psychiatry 2004;55:578–87. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.11.017&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15013826&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000220109100004&link_type=ISI) 100.Schaefer HS, Putnam KM, Benca RM, et al.Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging measures of neural activity to positive social stimuli in pre- and post-treatment depression.Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:974–86. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.024&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16780808&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000241691600011&link_type=ISI) 101.Almeida JR, Versace A, Mechelli A, et al.Abnormal amygdala-prefrontal effective connectivity to happy faces differentiates bi-polar from major depression.Biol Psychiatry 2009;66:451–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.024&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19450794&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000269330900007&link_type=ISI) 102.Almeida JR, Versace A, Hassel S, et al.Elevated amygdala activity to sad facial expressions: a state marker of bipolar but not unipolar depression.Biol Psychiatry 2010;67:414–21. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.027&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19931855&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000275100300004&link_type=ISI) 103.Lelli-Chiesa G, Kempton MJ, Jogia J, et al.The impact of the Val158Met catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype on neural correlates of sad facial affect processing in patients with bipolar disorder and their relatives.Psychol Med 2011;41:779–88. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0033291710001431&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20667170&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 104.Malhi GS, Lagopoulos J, Das P, et al.A functional MRI study of theory of mind in euthymic bipolar disorder patients.Bipolar Disord 2008;10:943–56. 105.Kim E, Jung YC, Ku J, et al.Reduced activation in the mirror neuron system during a virtual social cognition task in euthymic bipolar disorder.Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2009;33:1409–16. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.07.019&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19632283&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 106.Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, et al.Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception.Biol Psychiatry 2003;54:504–14. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00168-9&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12946879&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000184906100002&link_type=ISI) 107.Ochsner KN, Gross JJ.The cognitive control of emotion.Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9:242–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15866151&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000229466500010&link_type=ISI) 108.Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, Drevets WC.A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder.Mol Psychiatry 2008;13:829833–57. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/mp.2008.82&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18574483&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 109.1. Baumeister RF, 2. Vohs KD Ochsner KN, Gross JJ.Thinking makes it so: a socia cognitive neuroscience approach to emotion regulationBaumeister RF, Vohs KDHandbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and applicationsNew York (NY)Guilford Press2004229–55. 110.Phan KL, Fitzgerald DA, Nathan PJ, et al.Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:210–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.030&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15691521&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000226886400002&link_type=ISI) 111.Knight RT, Staines WR, Swick D, et al.Prefrontal cortex regulates inhibition and excitation in distributed neural networks.Acta Psychol (Amst) 1999;101:159–78. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00004-9&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10344184&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000080386800003&link_type=ISI) 112.Miller EK, Cohen JD.An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function.Annu Rev Neurosci 2001;24:167–202. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11283309&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000170109100007&link_type=ISI) 113.Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, et al.For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion.Neuroimage 2004;23:483–99. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15488398&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000224817100005&link_type=ISI) 114.Gusnard DA, Raichle ME.Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain.Nat Rev Neurosci 2001;2:685–94. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/35094500&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11584306&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000171454000015&link_type=ISI) 115.Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, et al.Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion.J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14:1215–29. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1162/089892902760807212&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12495527&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000179163500008&link_type=ISI) 116.Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, et al.Conflict monitoring and cognitive control.Psychol Rev 2001;108:624–52. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11488380&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000170892100006&link_type=ISI) 117.Anderson AK, Phelps EA.Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events.Nature 2001;411:305–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/35077083&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11357132&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000168710000047&link_type=ISI) 118.Morris JS, Ohman A, Dolan RJ.Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala.Nature 1998;393:467–70. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/30976&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9624001&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000074020000044&link_type=ISI) 119.Whalen PJ, Rauch SL, Etcoff NL, et al.Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge.J Neurosci 1998;18:411–8. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6Njoiam5ldXJvIjtzOjU6InJlc2lkIjtzOjg6IjE4LzEvNDExIjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTg6Ii9qcG4vMzcvMy8xNTQuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 120.LeDoux JE (1996) The emotional brain (Simon & Shuster, New York (NY)). 121.LeDoux JE.Emotion circuits in the brain.Annu Rev Neurosci 2000;23:155–84. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10845062&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000086730500006&link_type=ISI) 122.Leppänen JM.Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings.Curr Opin Psychiatry 2006;19:34–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/01.yco.0000191500.46411.00&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16612176&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000235795100006&link_type=ISI) 123.1. Tomkins SS, 2. Izard CE Ekman P.Communication through nonverbal behavior: a source of information about an interpersonal relationshipTomkins SS, Izard CEAffect, cognition, and personalityNew York (NY)Springer Press1965390–442. 124.Asthana HS, Mandal MK, Khurana H, et al.Visuospatial and affect recognition deficit in depression.J Affect Disord 1998;48:57–62. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0165-0327(97)00140-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9495602&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000071884800006&link_type=ISI) 125.Csukly G, Czobor P, Szily E, et al.Facial expression recognition in depressed subjects: the impact of intensity level and arousal dimension.J Nerv Ment Dis 2009;197:98–103. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181923f82&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19214044&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 126.Feinberg TE, Rifkin A, Schaffer C, et al.Facial discrimination and emotional recognition in schizophrenia and affective disorders.Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986;43:276–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800030094010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=3954548&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1986A284600009&link_type=ISI) 127.Mikhailova ES, Vladimirova TV, Iznak AF, et al.Abnormal recognition of facial expression of emotions in depressed patients with major depression disorder and schizotypal personality disorder.Biol Psychiatry 1996;40:697–705. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0006-3223(96)00032-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8894061&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1996VL69400002&link_type=ISI) 128.Persad SM, Polivy J.Differences between depressed and nondepressed individuals in the recognition of and response to facial emotional cues.J Abnorm Psychol 1993;102:358–68. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0021-843X.102.3.358&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8408947&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1993LU08000003&link_type=ISI) 129.Rubinow DR, Post RM.Impaired recognition of affect in facial expression in depressed patients.Biol Psychiatry 1992;31:947–53. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0006-3223(92)90120-O&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=1637932&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1992JD61000009&link_type=ISI) 130.Yoon KL, Joormann J, Gotlib IH.Judging the intensity of facial expressions of emotion: depression-related biases in the processing of positive affect.J Abnorm Psychol 2009;118:223–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/a0014658&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19222328&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 131.Karparova SP, Kersting A, Suslow T.Disengagement of attention from facial emotion in unipolar depression.Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2005;59:723–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01443.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16401250&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 132.Marcel BB, Samson J, Cole JO, et al.Discrimination of facial emotion in depressed patients with visual-perceptual disturbances.J Nerv Ment Dis 1993;181:583–4. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/00005053-199309000-00010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8245928&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 133.Sprengelmeyer R, Steele JD, Mwangi B, et al.The insular cortex and the neuroanatomy of major depression.J Affect Disord 2011;133:120–7. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2011.04.004&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21531027&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 134.Gotlib IH, Kasch KL, Traill S, et al.Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia.J Abnorm Psychol 2004;113:386–98. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0021-843X.113.3.386&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15311984&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000223137900003&link_type=ISI) 135.LeMoult J, Joormann J, Sherdell L, et al.Identification of emotional facial expressions following recovery from depression.J Abnorm Psychol 2009;118:828–33. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/a0016944&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19899852&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 136.Surguladze SA, Young AW, Senior C, et al.Recognition accuracy and response bias to happy and sad facial expressions in patients with major depression.Neuropsychology 2004;18:212–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0894-4105.18.2.212&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15099143&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000220725100002&link_type=ISI) 137.Suslow T, Junghanns K, Arolt V.Detection of facial expressions of emotions in depression.Percept Mot Skills 2001;92:857–68. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.2466/PMS.92.3.857-868&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11453215&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000169684300033&link_type=ISI) 138.Gilboa-Schechtman E, Erhard-Weiss D, Jeczemien P.Interpersonal deficits meet cognitive biases: memory for facial expressions in depressed and anxious men and women.Psychiatry Res 2002;113:279–93. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00266-4&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12559484&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000180881900007&link_type=ISI) 139.Harmer CJ, O’Sullivan U, Favaron E, et al.Effect of acute antidepressant administration on negative affective bias in depressed patients.Am J Psychiatry 2009;166:1178–84. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020149&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19755572&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000270389400019&link_type=ISI) 140.Levkovitz Y, Lamy D, Ternochiano P, et al.Perception of dyadic relationship and emotional states in patients with affective disorder.J Affect Disord 2003;75:19–28. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00024-1&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12781346&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 141.Suslow T, Dannlowski U, Lalee-Mentzel J, et al.Spatial processing of facial emotion in patients with unipolar depression: a longitudinal study.J Affect Disord 2004;83:59–63. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2004.03.003&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15546646&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000225913900007&link_type=ISI) 142.Flanagan TJ, White H, Carter BG.Differential impairments in emotion face recognition in postpartum and nonpostpartum depressed women.J Affect Disord 2011;128:314–8. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20800287&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 143.Goeleven E, De Raedt R, Baert S, et al.Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression.J Affect Disord 2006;93:149–57. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2006.03.007&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16647141&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000238773300018&link_type=ISI) 144.Gur RC, Erwin RJ, Gur RE, et al.Facial emotion discrimination: II. Behavioral findings in depression.Psychiatry Res 1992;42:241–51. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0165-1781(92)90116-K&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=1496056&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1992JD71700005&link_type=ISI) 145.Mandal MK, Bhattacharya BB.Recognition of facial affect in depression.Percept Mot Skills 1985;61:13–4. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.2466/pms.1985.61.1.13&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=4047873&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 146.Milders M, Bell S, Platt J, et al.Stable expression recognition abnormalities in unipolar depression.Psychiatry Res 2010;179:38–42. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.psychres.2009.05.015&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20478626&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000281368300007&link_type=ISI) 147.Fritzsche A, Dahme B, Gotlib IH, et al.Specificity of cognitive biases in patients with current depression and remitted depression and in patients with asthma.Psychol Med 2010;40:815–26. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0033291709990948&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19719897&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 148.Gollan JK, McCloskey M, Hoxha D, et al.How do depressed and healthy adults interpret nuanced facial expressions?.J Abnorm Psychol 2010;119:804–10. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/a0020234&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20939654&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 149.Gaebel W, Wolwer W.Facial expression and emotional face recognition in schizophrenia and depression.Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1992;242:46–52. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/BF02190342&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=1390955&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1992JN48900008&link_type=ISI) 150.Hale WW III., Jansen JH, Bouhuys AL, et al.The judgement of facial expressions by depressed patients, their partners and controls.J Affect Disord 1998;47:63–70. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0165-0327(97)00112-2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9476745&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 151.Koschack J, Hoschel K, Irle E.Differential impairments of facial affect priming in subjects with acute or partially remitted major depressive episodes.J Nerv Ment Dis 2003;191:175–81. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12637844&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 152.Naranjo C, Kornreich C, Campanella S, et al.Major depression is associated with impaired processing of emotion in music as well as in facial and vocal stimuli.J Affect Disord 2011;128:243–51. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2010.06.039&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20663569&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 153.Bouhuys AL, Bos EH, Geerts E, et al.The association between levels of cortisol secretion and fear perception in patients with remitted depression predicts recurrence.J Nerv Ment Dis 2006;194:478–84. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/01.nmd.0000228502.52864.ce&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16840843&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 154.Bouhuys AL, Geerts E, Mersch PP.Relationship between perception of facial emotions and anxiety in clinical depression: Does anxiety-related perception predict persistence of depression?.J Affect Disord 1997;43:213–23. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0165-0327(97)01432-8&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9186792&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 155.Sterzer P, Hilgenfeldt T, Freudenberg P, et al.Access of emotional information to visual awareness in patients with major depressive disorder.Psychol Med 2011;41:1615–24. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0033291710002540&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21208495&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 156.Gilboa-Schechtman E, Foa E, Vaknin Y, et al.Interpersonal sensitivity and response bias in social phobia and depression: labeling emotional expressions.Cognit Ther Res 2008;32:605–18. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s10608-008-9208-8&link_type=DOI) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000259513900001&link_type=ISI) 157.David AS, Cutting JC.Affect, affective disorder and schizophrenia. A neuropsychological investigation of right hemisphere function.Br J Psychiatry 1990;156:491–5. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6ImJqcHJjcHN5Y2giO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6OToiMTU2LzQvNDkxIjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTg6Ii9qcG4vMzcvMy8xNTQuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 158.Leppänen JM, Milders M, Bell JS, et al.Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces.Psychiatry Res 2004;128:123–33. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.020&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15488955&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000224994600003&link_type=ISI) 159.Wright SL, Langenecker SA, Deldin PJ, et al.Gender-specific disruptions in emotion processing in younger adults with depression.Depress Anxiety 2009;26:182–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/da.20502&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18800371&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000263307500011&link_type=ISI) 160.Joormann J, Gotlib IH.Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression.J Abnorm Psychol 2007;116:80–5. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.80&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17324018&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000244162200007&link_type=ISI) 161.Leyman L, De Raedt R, Schacht R, et al.Attentional biases for angry faces in unipolar depression.Psychol Med 2007;37:393–402. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S003329170600910X&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17076914&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000245059300009&link_type=ISI) 162.Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, et al.Neurobiology of emotion perception II: implications for major psychiatric disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2003;54:515–28. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00171-9&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12946880&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000184906100003&link_type=ISI) 163.Wilbertz G, Brakemeier EL, Zobel I, et al.Exploring preoperational features in chronic depression.J Affect Disord 2010;124:262–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2009.11.021&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20089311&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 164.Bazin N, Brunet-Gouet E, Bourdet C, et al.Quantitative assessment of attribution of intentions to others in schizophrenia using an ecological video-based task: a comparison with manic and depressed patients.Psychiatry Res 2009;167:28–35. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19346006&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 165.MacQueen GM, Galway TM, Hay J, et al.Recollection memory deficits in patients with major depressive disorder predicted by past depressions but not current mood state or treatment status.Psychol Med 2002;32:251–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0033291701004834&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11866320&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000173990900007&link_type=ISI) 166.Basso MR, Bornstein RA.Relative memory deficits in recurrent versus first-episode major depression on a word-list learning task.Neuropsychology 1999;13:557–63. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0894-4105.13.4.557&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10527064&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000083035700010&link_type=ISI) 167.Lebowitz BK, Shear PK, Steed MA, et al.Verbal fluency in mania: relationship to number of manic episodes.Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 2001;14:177–82. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11513101&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000170412500006&link_type=ISI) 168.Milne AM, Macqueen GM, Hall GB.Abnormal hippocampal activation in patients with extensive history of major depression: an fMRI study.J Psychiatry Neurosci 2011;36:110004 169.van Gorp WG, Altshuler L, Theberge DC, et al.Cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar patients with and without prior alcohol dependence. A preliminary study.Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998;55:41–6. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.55.1.41&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9435759&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000071301900009&link_type=ISI) 170.Amaral DG.The primate amygdala and the neurobiology of social behavior: implications for understanding social anxiety.Biol Psychiatry 2002;51:11–7. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01307-5&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11801227&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000173330000003&link_type=ISI) 171.Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, et al.A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62:146–52. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.146&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15699291&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000226742300004&link_type=ISI) 172.Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Tekell JL, et al.Regional metabolic effects of fluoxetine in major depression: serial changes and relationship to clinical response.Biol Psychiatry 2000;48:830–43. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01036-2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11063978&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000165056200012&link_type=ISI) 173.Nobler MS, Oquendo MA, Kegeles LS, et al.Decreased regional brain metabolism after ect.Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:305–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.158.2.305&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11156816&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000166761400023&link_type=ISI) 174.Ehrenreich H, Hinze-Selch D, Stawicki S, et al.Improvement of cognitive functions in chronic schizophrenic patients by recombinant human erythropoietin.Mol Psychiatry 2007;12:206–20. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/sj.mp.4001907&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17033631&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000243783700009&link_type=ISI) 175.Harmer CJ.Serotonin and emotional processing: Does it help explain antidepressant drug action?.Neuropharmacology 2008;55:1023–8. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.036&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18634807&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000260982700011&link_type=ISI) 176.Gilboa A, Shalev AY, Laor L, et al.Functional connectivity of the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala in posttraumatic stress disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2004;55:263–72. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.08.004&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14744467&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000188434000009&link_type=ISI) 177.Shin LM, Wright CI, Cannistraro PA, et al.A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62:273–81. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.273&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15753240&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000227391600005&link_type=ISI) 178.Drevets WC, Raichle ME.Neuroanatomical circuits in depression: implications for treatment mechanisms.Psychopharmacol Bull 1992;28:261–74. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=1480730&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1992JY72100012&link_type=ISI) 179.Mayberg HS, Lozano AM, Voon V, et al.Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression.Neuron 2005;45:651–60. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.014&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15748841&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000227446700005&link_type=ISI) 180.Getz GE, Shear PK, Strakowski SM.Facial affect recognition deficits in bipolar disorder.J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2003;9:623–32. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S1355617703940021&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12755174&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000182725700003&link_type=ISI) 181.Lembke A, Ketter TA.Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania.Am J Psychiatry 2002;159:302–4. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.302&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11823275&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000173727500020&link_type=ISI) 182.McClure EB, Treland JE, Snow J, et al.Deficits in social cognition and response flexibility in pediatric bipolar disorder.Am J Psychiatry 2005;162:1644–51. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1644&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16135623&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000231559300011&link_type=ISI) 183.Douglas KM, Porter RJ.Recognition of disgusted facial expressions in severe depression.Br J Psychiatry 2010;197:156–7. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6ImJqcHJjcHN5Y2giO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6OToiMTk3LzIvMTU2IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTg6Ii9qcG4vMzcvMy8xNTQuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 184.McClure EB, Pope K, Hoberman AJ, et al.Facial expression recognition in adolescents with mood and anxiety disorders.Am J Psychiatry 2003;160:1172–4. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1172&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12777278&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000183240800023&link_type=ISI) 185.Schaefer KL, Baumann J, Rich BA, et al.Perception of facial emotion in adults with bipolar or unipolar depression and controls.J Psychiatr Res 2010;44:1229–35. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20510425&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 186.Gray J, Venn H, Montagne B, et al.Bipolar patients show mood-congruent biases in sensitivity to facial expressions of emotion when exhibiting depressed symptoms, but not when exhibiting manic symptoms.Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2006;11:505–20. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1080/13546800544000028&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17354085&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 187.Summers M, Papadopoulou K, Bruno S, et al.Bipolar I and bipolar II disorder: cognition and emotion processing.Psychol Med 2006;36:1799–809. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0033291706008804&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16938147&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000243764200015&link_type=ISI) 188.Chen CH, Lennox B, Jacob R, et al.Explicit and implicit facial affect recognition in manic and depressed states of bipolar disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Biol Psychiatry 2006;59:31–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.008&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16112653&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000234197800005&link_type=ISI) 189.Brotman MA, Guyer AE, Lawson ES, et al.Facial emotion labeling deficits in children and adolescents at risk for bipolar disorder.Am J Psychiatry 2008;165:385–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06122050&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18245180&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000253779400019&link_type=ISI) 190.Dickstein DP, Leibenluft E.Emotion regulation in children and adolescents: boundaries between normalcy and bipolar disorder.Dev Psychopathol 2006;18:1105–31. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17064430&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000241933300008&link_type=ISI) 191.McClure-Tone EB.Socioemotional functioning in bipolar disorder versus typical development: behavioral and neural differences.Clin Psychol (New York) 2009;16:98–113. 192.Mayberg HS.Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression.J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997;9:471–81. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/jnp.9.3.471&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9276848&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1997XR78200013&link_type=ISI) 193.Derntl B, Seidel EM, Kryspin-Exner I, et al.Facial emotion recognition in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder.Br J Clin Psychol 2009;48:363–75. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1348/014466509X404845&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19220936&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 194.Brotman MA, Skup M, Rich BA, et al.Risk for bipolar disorder is associated with face-processing deficits across emotions.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008;47:1455–61. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/CHI.0b013e318188832e&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=19034190&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 195.Guyer AE, McClure EB, Adler AD, et al.Specificity of facial expression labeling deficits in childhood psychopathology.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007;48:863–71. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01758.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17714371&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000249130800003&link_type=ISI) 196.Mitchell RL, Elliott R, Barry M, et al.The neural response to emotional prosody, as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging.Neuropsychologia 2003;41:1410–21. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00017-4&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12757912&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000183368300012&link_type=ISI) 197.Pincus D, Kose S, Arana A, et al.Inverse effects of oxytocin on attributing mental activity to others in depressed and healthy subjects: a double-blind placebo controlled FMRI study.Front Psychiatry 2010;1:134 [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21423444&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 198.Ross ED.The aprosodias. Functional-anatomic organization of the affective components of language in the right hemisphere.Arch Neurol 1981;38:561–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archneur.1981.00510090055006&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7271534&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1981ME56900005&link_type=ISI) 199.Wildgruber D, Ackermann H, Kreifelts B, et al.Cerebral processing of linguistic and emotional prosody: fMRI studies.Prog Brain Res 2006;156:249–68. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56013-3&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17015084&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000247001200013&link_type=ISI) 200.Buchanan TW, Lutz K, Mirzazade S, et al.Recognition of emotional prosody and verbal components of spoken language: an fMRI study.Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2000;9:227–38. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00060-9&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10808134&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 201.Kotz SA, Meyer M, Alter K, et al.On the lateralization of emotional prosody: an event-related functional MR investigation.Brain Lang 2003;86:366–76. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00532-1&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12972367&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000185546500002&link_type=ISI) 202.Wildgruber D, Riecker A, Hertrich I, et al.Identification of emotional intonation evaluated by fMRI.Neuroimage 2005;24:1233–41. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.034&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15670701&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000226788100032&link_type=ISI) 203.Wildgruber D, Hertrich I, Riecker A, et al.Distinct frontal regions subserve evaluation of linguistic and emotional aspects of speech intonation.Cereb Cortex 2004;14:1384–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/cercor/bhh099&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15217896&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000225077400010&link_type=ISI) 204.Adolphs R, Damasio H, Tranel D.Neural systems for recognition of emotional prosody: a 3-D lesion study.Emotion 2002;2:23–51. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/1528-3542.2.1.23&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12899365&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000208224400003&link_type=ISI) 205.Kan Y, Mimura M, Kamijima K, et al.Recognition of emotion from moving facial and prosodic stimuli in depressed patients.J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004;75:1667–71. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoiam5ucCI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMDoiNzUvMTIvMTY2NyI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjE4OiIvanBuLzM3LzMvMTU0LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 206.Murphy D, Cutting J.Prosodic comprehension and expression in schizophrenia.J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1990;53:727–30. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoiam5ucCI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czo4OiI1My85LzcyNyI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjE4OiIvanBuLzM3LzMvMTU0LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 207.Péron J, El Tamer S, Grandjean D, et al.Major depressive disorder skews the recognition of emotional prosody.Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2011;35:987–96. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.01.019&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21296120&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 208.Uekermann J, Abdel-Hamid M, Lehmkamper C, et al.Perception of affective prosody in major depression: A link to executive functions?.J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;14:552–61. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18577284&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 209.Bozikas VP, Kosmidis MH, Tonia T, et al.Impaired perception of affective prosody in remitted patients with bipolar disorder.J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007;19:436–40. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1176/appi.neuropsych.19.4.436&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18070847&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000251216700011&link_type=ISI) 210.Premack D, Woodruff G.Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?.Behav Brain Sci 1978;1:515–26. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0140525X00076512&link_type=DOI) 211.Leslie AM, Friedman O, German TP.Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’.Trends Cogn Sci 2004;8:528–33. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.001&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15556021&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000202949800004&link_type=ISI) 212.McKinnon MC, Moscovitch M.Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored.Cognition 2007;102:179–218. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.011&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16480974&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000243818900002&link_type=ISI) 213.Channon S, Crawford S.The effects of anterior lesions on performance on a story comprehension test: left anterior impairment on a theory of mind-type task.Neuropsychologia 2000;38:1006–17. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00154-2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10775711&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000087160700011&link_type=ISI) 214.Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, et al.Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage.Cogn Behav Neurol 2005;18:55–67. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/01.wnn.0000152228.90129.99&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15761277&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000229236600007&link_type=ISI) 215.Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S.The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences.J Autism Dev Disord 2004;34:163–75. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15162935&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000220600700007&link_type=ISI) 216.Fletcher PC, Happé F, Frith U, et al.Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension.Cognition 1995;57:109–28. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0010-0277(95)00692-R&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8556839&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1995TH57500001&link_type=ISI) 217.Goel V, Jordan G, Sadato N, et al.Modeling other minds.Neuroreport 1995;6:1741–6. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/00001756-199509000-00009&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8541472&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1995RW47800009&link_type=ISI) 218.Rowe AD, Bullock PR, Polkey CE, et al.“Theory of mind” impairments and their relationship to executive functioning following frontal lobe excisions”.Brain 2001;124:600–16. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/brain/124.3.600&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11222459&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000167465100015&link_type=ISI) 219.Stuss DT, Gallup GG, Alexander MP.The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’.Brain 2001;124:279–86. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/brain/124.2.279&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11157555&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000167059700003&link_type=ISI) 220.Gallagher HL, Happé F, Brunswick N, et al.Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks.Neuropsychologia 2000;38:11–21. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00053-6&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10617288&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000084135500002&link_type=ISI) 221.McCabe K, Houser D, Ryan L, et al.A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001;98:11832–5. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoicG5hcyI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMToiOTgvMjAvMTE4MzIiO3M6NDoiYXRvbSI7czoxODoiL2pwbi8zNy8zLzE1NC5hdG9tIjt9czo4OiJmcmFnbWVudCI7czowOiIiO30=) 222.1. Roberts MJ McKinnon MC, Levine B, Moscovitch M.Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: the perspective from cognitive neuroscienceRoberts MJIntegrating the mindHove (UK)Psychology Press2007153–77. 223.Aichhorn M, Perner J, Kronbichler M, et al.Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind?.Neuroimage 2006;30:1059–68. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.026&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16337813&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000236894800039&link_type=ISI) 224.Blanke O, Arzy S.The out-of-body experience: disturbed self-processing at the temporoparietal junction.Neuroscientist 2005;11:16–24. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/1073858404270885&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15632275&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000226405800010&link_type=ISI) 225.Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, Michelich CR, et al.Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements.Cereb Cortex 2005;15:1866–76. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/cercor/bhi064&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15746001&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000233217300004&link_type=ISI) 226.Pelphrey KA, Viola RJ, McCarthy G.When strangers pass: processing of mutual and averted social gaze in the superior temporal sulcus.Psychol Sci 2004;15:598–603. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00726.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15327630&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000223404300005&link_type=ISI) 227.Saxe R, Carey S, Kanwisher N.Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging.Annu Rev Psychol 2004;55:87–124. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142044&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14744211&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000189351100004&link_type=ISI) 228.Samson D, Apperly IA, Chiavarino C, et al.Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else’s belief.Nat Neurosci 2004;7:499–500. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nn1223&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15077111&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000221101300026&link_type=ISI) 229.Saxe R, Kanwisher N.People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporoparietal junction in “theory of mind”.Neuroimage 2003;19:1835–42. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12948738&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000185079000053&link_type=ISI) 230.Brüne M, Brüne-Cohrs U.Theory of mind-evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology.Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30:437–55. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.001&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16239031&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000238464500001&link_type=ISI) 231.Inoue Y, Yamada K, Kanba S.Deficit in theory of mind is a risk for relapse of major depression.J Affect Disord 2006;95:125–7. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2006.04.018&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16797082&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 232.Lee L, Harkness KL, Sabbagh MA, et al.Mental state decoding abilities in clinical depression.J Affect Disord 2005;86:247–58. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2005.02.007&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15935244&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000229879400015&link_type=ISI) 233.Wang YG, Wang YQ, Chen SL, et al.Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: a componential view.Psychiatry Res 2008;161:153–61. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.018&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18926572&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000261228200003&link_type=ISI) 234.Kettle JW, O’Brien-Simpson L, Allen NB.Impaired theory of mind in first-episode schizophrenia: comparison with community, university and depressed controls.Schizophr Res 2008;99:96–102. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.011&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18155447&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000254306600012&link_type=ISI) 235.Wolkenstein L, Schonenberg M, Schirm E, et al.I can see what you feel, but I can’t deal with it: impaired theory of mind in depression.J Affect Disord 2011;132:104–11. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=21420177&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 236.Dziobek I, Fleck S, Kalbe E, et al.Introducing MASC: a movie for the assessment of social cognition.J Autism Dev Disord 2006;36:623–36. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/s10803-006-0107-0&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16755332&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000239927900006&link_type=ISI) 237.McKinnon MC, Cusi AM, MacQueen GM.Impaired theory of mind performance in patients with recurrent bipolar disorder: moderating effect of cognitive load.Psychiatry Res 2010;177:261–2. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20223523&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 238.Inoue Y, Tonooka Y, Yamada K, et al.Deficiency of theory of mind in patients with remitted mood disorder.J Affect Disord 2004;82:403–9. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15555691&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 239.Lahera G, Montes JM, Benito A, et al.Theory of mind deficit in bipolar disorder: Is it related to a previous history of psychotic symptoms?.Psychiatry Res 2008;161:309–17. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18996602&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 240.Olley AL, Malhi GS, Bachelor J, et al.Executive functioning and theory of mind in euthymic bipolar disorder.Bipolar Disord 2005;7Suppl 543–52. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00254.x&link_type=DOI) 241.Perner J, Aichhorn M.Theory of mind, language and the temporoparietal junction mystery.Trends Cogn Sci 2008;12:123–6. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.001&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18329322&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000255469200001&link_type=ISI) 242.Rizzolatti G, Craighero L.The mirror-neuron system.Annu Rev Neurosci 2004;27:169–92. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=15217330&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000223246300007&link_type=ISI) 243.Ressler KJ, Mayberg HS.Targeting abnormal neural circuits in mood and anxiety disorders: from the laboratory to the clinic.Nat Neurosci 2007;10:1116–24. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nn1944&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17726478&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000249144000010&link_type=ISI) 244.Blakemore SJ.The social brain in adolescence.Nat Rev Neurosci 2008;9:267–77. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1038/nrn2353&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18354399&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000254145200013&link_type=ISI) 245.1. Kurtimes WM, 2. Gewirtz JL Hoffman ML.Empathy, social cognition, and moral actionKurtimes WM, Gewirtz JLHandbook of moral behavior and developmentHillsdale (NJ)Lawrence Erlbaum1991275–301. 246.1. Adams GR, 2. Berzonsky MD , eds (2003) The Blackwell handbook of adolescence (Blackwell, Oxford (UK)). 247.Choudhury S, Blakemore SJ, Charman T.Social cognitive development during adolescence.Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2006;1:165–74. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/scan/nsl024&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18985103&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) 248.Fleming JE, Offord DR.Epidemiology of childhood depressive disorders: a critical review.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1990;29:571–80. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1097/00004583-199007000-00010&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=2201675&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1990DP22900010&link_type=ISI) 249.Lewinsohn PM, Hops H, Roberts RE, et al.Adolescent psychopathology: I. Prevalence and incidence of depression and other DSM-III-R disorders in high school students.J Abnorm Psychol 1993;102:133–44. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/0021-843X.102.1.133&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8436689&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1993KL72800014&link_type=ISI) 250.Merikangas KR, Akiskal HS, Angst J, et al.Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of bipolar spectrum disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey replication.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007;64:543–52. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.543&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17485606&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000246263300004&link_type=ISI) 251.Bediou B, Krolak-Salmon P, Saoud M, et al.Facial expression and sex recognition in schizophrenia and depression.Can J Psychiatry 2005;50:525–33. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1177/070674370505000905&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16262107&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F3%2F154.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000234709200005&link_type=ISI)