Nonlinear analysis of electroencephalogram at rest and during cognitive tasks in patients with schizophrenia ============================================================================================================ * Elisa Carlino * Monica Sigaudo * Antonella Pollo * Fabrizio Benedetti * Tullia Mongini * Filomena Castagna * Sergio Vighetti * Paola Rocca ## Abstract **Background:** In spite of the large number of studies on schizophrenia, a full understanding of its core pathology still eludes us. The application of the nonlinear theory of electroencephalography (EEG) analysis provides an interesting tool to differentiate between physiologic conditions (e.g., resting state and mathematical task) and normal and pathologic brain activities. The aim of the present study was to investigate nonlinear EEG activity in patients with schizophrenia. **Methods:** We recorded 19-lead EEGs in patients with stable schizophrenia and healthy controls under 4 different conditions: eyes closed, eyes open, forward counting and backward counting. A nonlinear measure of complexity was calculated by means of correlation dimension (D2). **Results:** We included 17 patients and 17 controls in our analysis. Comparing the 2 populations, we observed greater D2 values in the patient group. In controls, increased D2 values were observed during active states (eyes open and the 2 cognitive tasks) compared with baseline conditions. This increase of brain complexity, which can be interpreted as an increase of information processing and integration, was not preserved in the patient population. **Limitations:** Patients with schizophrenia were taking antipsychotic medications, so the presence of medication effects cannot be excluded. **Conclusion:** Our results suggest that patients with schizophrenia present changes in brain activity compared with healthy controls, and this pathologic alteration can be successfully studied with nonlinear EEG analysis. ## Introduction The precise mechanisms underlying schizophrenia remain poorly understood, even though current pathophysiologic theories suggest that its core pathology is an abnormal functional integration of the neural system (disconnection hypothesis).1 Numerous electroencephalographic (EEG) studies have been conducted to characterize the brain activity of patients with schizophrenia. Different nonspecific abnormalities have been reported, but it is generally agreed that no characteristic pattern is apparent on visual inspection of EEGs.2,3 However, recent applications of the nonlinear theory of EEG have provided a new, potentially interesting research tool.4 Nonlinear EEG patterns have been reported in different physiologic conditions, such as sleep,5 and in pathologic states, such as epilepsy and dementia.6–9 Nonlinear EEG patterns in patients with schizophrenia have also been described.10,11 In 2006, Breakspear12 suggested that the study of nonlinearity in the field of schizophrenia could be justified at a neurophysiologic level (the nonlinear dynamic properties of neural systems), at a clinical level (the dynamic nature and the fluctuations of symptoms) and from a conceptual point of view (psychotic symptoms as a failure of the stability of nonlinear brain systems). One approach to apply nonlinear methods to the analysis of EEGs is to estimate the dimensional complexity of the signal through the correlation dimension (D2),13 which can be defined as the number of independent variables necessary to describe the behaviour of a dynamic system.14 The neurophysiologic meaning of D2 is not clear. However, since EEGs reflect cortical dynamics, D2 is often interpreted as a measure of complexity (or flexibility) of information processing, and it can be interpreted as an index of the integration of information in the brain. Nonlinear analysis of EEGs, namely D2 estimates, has proven to be very useful in comparing different physiologic states. A study conducted in young healthy volunteers demonstrated that the dimensional complexity of EEGs, as measured by D2, was greater while participants solved tasks of divergent (creative) rather than convergent thinking; in turn, the D2 values were greater during convergent thinking than mental relaxation.15 Decreased D2 values have been detected in a variety of clinical and experimental conditions presenting grossly impaired information processing. Babloyantz and Destexhe16 demonstrated low-dimensional dynamics in the EEG of a patient with Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease. Lower D2 values have been detected in healthy men in a state of total sleep deprivation,17 and lower values have also been demonstrated in patients with epilepsy during an absence seizure.18 Moreover, D2 has been extensively investigated in patients with Alzheimer disease,7,19,20 and results have shown a decrease in values in the patient population, with a correlation between D2 and severity of dementia. These data suggest the possible use of this measure in the assessment of patients with Alzheimer disease, with improved accuracy of diagnosis.9 Similarly, the possible usefulness of D2 values in a clinical setting has been supported by investigations in patients with Parkinson disease.8,21 Several studies have investigated D2 in patients with schizophrenia.2,10,22,23 Some differences in EEG complexity between these patients and healthy controls have been reported, with contradictory findings likely owing to the wide variations in experimental conditions and sample selection.24 However, to our knowledge, EEG patterns during resting and active conditions have not been compared yet, thus no information regarding the brain activity during different conditions is available. Despite the widespread application of D2, limitations of nonlinear approaches to time series analysis have been recognized. In particular, there is the question of whether EEG time series contain nonlinear properties. To answer this question, control methods to test nonlinearity have been developed. One of the most popular methods is surrogate data testing.25,26 Surrogate data are constructed to preserve the same linear properties (power spectrum/autocorrelation function) as the original time series but present different nonlinear dynamics. With this method, evidence of nonlinear dynamics has been detected in healthy participants27 and in patients with epilepsy,28 dementia and Parkinson disease.29,30 However, for patients with schizophrenia, results in the field of EEG nonlinearity are still inconclusive.11 Taking these considerations into account, the aim of the present study was to investigate the dimensional complexity of EEG by means of D2 values in patients with schizophrenia. We sought to record 19-lead EEGs in a sample of patients with stable schizophrenia and healthy volunteers. An extensive protocol registration was developed, with a resting condition (eyes closed) and 3 active conditions (eyes open, counting forward and counting backward). We explored D2 changes from resting to active conditions. Furthermore, we investigated the presence of nonlinearity in EEG using a surrogate data technique. ## Methods ### Participants We recruited patients with schizophrenia from the outpatient clinics at the Department of Neuroscience, Section of Psychiatry, and the Department of Mental Health ASL 1 Ospedale San Giovanni Battista, Turin, Italy. They all fulfilled formal *Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders* (DSM-IV-TR)31 criteria for schizophrenia. The diagnosis was confirmed by 2 expert clinicians using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID).32 At the time of study entry, patients had been clinically stable for at least 6 months, as judged by the treating psychiatrist. This means that during this period, all patients could be treated as outpatients, treatment regimen was not modified, and there was no essential change in psychopathology. Patients were evaluated using a semistructured interview to assess demographic and clinical features. We collected data on age, sex, education, vocational functioning, age at onset of schizophrenia (report of first contact with a psychiatric service) and duration of illness. Participants were excluded if they had a current Axis I disorder other than schizophrenia (as determined with the SCID), a current or past comorbid diagnosis of an autism-spectrum disorder or another pervasive developmental disorder, a history of severe head injury (coma ≥ 48 h) or a diagnosis of a psychiatric disorder owing to a general medical condition. All patients were taking a second-generation antipsychotic at the time of assessment. We recruited a group of healthy matched controls without history of sustained head injury or other neurologic or psychiatric disorders. Controls were recruited from the community and from the local university. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after a complete description of the study. We determined whether patients with schizophrenia were capable of providing informed consent on the basis of the clinical interview and with the help of information obtained by the treating psychiatrist. The study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the ethics committee of our hospital (Comitato Etico Interaziendale A.O.U. San Giovanni Battista di Torino, A.O. C.T.O. Maria Adelaide di Torino). ### Psychiatric and neuropsychologic assessment We rated overall severity of illness using the Clinical Global Impression–Severity Scale (CGI-S),33 and current levels of psychopathologic symptoms were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), a rater-administered 30-item scale for measuring positive symptoms, negative symptoms and general psychopathology subscales.36 The Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) scale was used to quantify patients’ psychologic, social and occupational/educational functioning.35 Neuropsychologic tests were administered by 1 trained psychologist who was unaware of participants’ clinical characteristics or scores on the psychiatric rating scales. The battery was administered and scored on the day after the psychiatric assessments. The total testing time was 1–2 hours per patient. No participants were familiar with the tests, and they all underwent a neuropsychologic battery assessing attention, verbal memory and executive functioning. To evaluate attentive functioning, we used the number of colours named in the conflicting card of the Stroop Test,37 which is a specific index of the interference sensitivity and/or response inhibition. Moreover, we calculated the time measured in seconds of Part B minus Part A of the Trail Making Test (TMT)38 to assess divided attention and set shifting. Verbal memory was assessed using the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT),34 and we recorded the total number of items correctly recalled over 5 learning trials (CVLT 1–5). To assess executive functioning, the number of achieved sorting categories of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) was used.39 ### Electroencephalogram recording Participants sat in a comfortable chair, and EEG data were recorded using a 19-lead EEG (Galileo; EBNeuro S.p.A.). Nineteen electrodes were applied to the scalp in accordance with the 10–20 international system (Fz, F1, F2, F3, F4, F7, F8, C3, C4, Cz, P3, P4, Pz, T3, T4 T5, T6, O1, O2) with linked common ear reference. Impedance was less than 2 KΩ in each active lead. Data were collected and digitized at a sampling rate of 1024 Hz, with a low frequency filter of 3 Hz and a high frequency filter of 30 Hz in each channel. The EEG was recorded in 4 different conditions: a baseline period with eyes closed, an active period with eyes open and 2 cognitive task periods (counting forward and counting backward). Recordings started with 5 minutes with eyes closed followed by 4 minutes with eyes open, during which participants were asked to keep their eyes steady on an object positioned in front of them. After another 2 minutes with eyes closed, the 45 second cognitive tasks started: participants were asked to count forward in steps of 3 starting from 1 and, after another 2 minutes with eyes closed, they were asked to count backward in steps of 3 starting from 100. Recordings ended with 2 minutes with eyes closed (Fig. 1). All EEG scans were visually inspected offline by 3 independent raters to discard EEG artifacts, and 16-second epochs (16 384 data points) without artifacts were selected for each condition. Data from the T5 electrode were excluded from the analysis for technical problems evidenced in all the recordings during the data analysis. ![Fig. 1](http://jpn.ca/https://www.jpn.ca/content/jpn/37/4/259/F1.medium.gif) [Fig. 1](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/F1) Fig. 1 Experimental protocol. ### Nonlinear and statistical analyses To test the nonlinearity of the EEGs, we used the surrogate data method.25,26 After the Fourier transform, the EEG phases were randomized and then the inverse Fourier transform was computed, returning to the time domain. Subsequently, we calculated the nonlinear measure (D2) on the surrogate data and compared it with the original values using *z* scores.40 Only *z* scores greater than 1.96 (*p* < 0.05) were considered to be significant. We obtained D2 values with a specific program (Complexity v.2.0; Laxtha Inc.). The optimal values of embedding dimension and delay number were automatically set: we used an embedding dimension of 9 and time delays of 5–15 ms. Computation details are described in Appendix 1, available at cma.ca/jpn. Four different regions were considered and designated as follows: frontal (average of Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8), central (average of C3, Cz, C4), temporal (average of T3, T4, T5, T6) and occipital (average of P3, Pz, P4, O1, O2). We performed a mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) between groups using the Dunnet post hoc test for multiple comparisons. Data are presented as means and standard deviations (SD), and we considered results to be significant at *p* < 0.05. The analysis was performed with Statistica, version 9, for Windows. ## Results We recruited 23 patients with schizophrenia for the study. Of these, 6 patients were excluded owing to unanalyzable EEG recording (EEG interference, muscular artifacts; *n* = 2), inability to follow the instructions of the experimental protocol (*n* = 3) and protocol interruption (*n* = 1). Data for 17 patients (10 men, 7 women, mean age 34.7 [SD 10.3] yr, mean education 12.1 [SD 3.2] yr) and 17 age- and sex-matched controls without history of psychiatric or neurologic disease (9 men, 8 women, mean age 36.5 [SD 13.9] yr, mean education 15.8 [SD 4.1] yr) were available for analysis. The demographic and clinical characteristics of participants are summarized in Table 1. All patients were right-handed, whereas 1 control was left-handed. Patients and controls did not differ in age (*t* = −0.434, *p* = 0.67) or sex (χ21 = 0.000, *p* > 0.99) but a difference in education was observed, with a higher level in the control population (*t* = −2.926, *p* = 0.006; Table 1). However, the education level was not correlated with D2 variables in either group (*p* > 0.09 in all cases). The antipsychotic medications that patients were taking at the time of assessment are listed in Table 2. View this table: [Table 1](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/T1) Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients with schizophrenia and healthy matched controls View this table: [Table 2](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/T2) Table 2 Antipsychotic medications taken by patients with schizophrenia enrolled in the study We checked the accuracy of the cognitive task by assessing differences in right/wrong answers in the counting forward and counting backward conditions. No significant difference was found (χ21 = 3.220, *p* = 0.07 for forward counting and χ21 = 1.275, *p* = 0.26 for backward counting). Furthermore, no correlation was observed between medication (expressed in chlorpromazine [CPZ] equivalents) and D2 values (r = −0.41, *p* = 0.09 for the eyes closed condition; r = −0.25, *p* = 0.33 for the eyes open condition; r = −0.38, *p* = 0.13 for the counting forward condition; and r = −0.28, *p* = 0.26 for the counting backward condition). The patient group showed a moderate severity of symptoms assessed by PANSS (mean 58.2 [SD 19.4] points) and CGI-S (mean 4.0 [SD 0.8] points). Moreover, the GAF results (mean 65.3 [SD 14.3] points) reflected an intermediate but significant level of functioning impairment. As far as the neuropsychologic assessment was concerned, controls performed better than patients on the CVLT (mean 66.2 [SD 6.5] points v. mean 44.4 [SD 10.2] points, *t* = 7.08, *p* < 0.001, Cohen *d* = 2.55), TMT B–A (mean 34.6 [SD 25.1] points v. mean 59.0 [SD 21.9] points, *t* = −2.81, *p* = 0.009, Cohen *d* = 1.04) and Stroop tests (mean 34.4 [SD 9.8] points v. mean 28.1 [SD 6.9] points, *t* = 2.16, *p* = 0.040, Cohen *d* = 0.80). The WCST results (controls: 6 categories; patients: 5.6 ± 0.4 categories) were similar for both groups. ### Surrogate data Globally, as revealed by the *z* scores, the original signals and the surrogate data differed in both groups in most conditions, suggesting the presence of nonlinear dynamics. In particular, in the control group the surrogate and the original D2 values differed in all conditions with the exception of frontal and central values for the counting forward condition. As for the patients, no differences were found in frontal, central and parietal values for the eyes closed condition or in frontal values for the eyes open condition. Accordingly, the above-listed conditions were excluded from the analysis and only the following 10 variables were considered: eyes closed (occipital), eyes open (central, parietal and occipital), counting forward (parietal and occipital) and counting backward (frontal, central, parietal and occipital). ### Between-group analysis The D2 values for each condition are reported in Table 3, Figure 2 and Figure 3 (individual electrodes) and in Table 4 (regions). A between-group analysis using a mixed ANOVA design was computed to investigate group differences. We observed a significant group effect (*F*1,32 = 5.056, *p* = 0.031; Fig. 4), with an increase of D2 values in the control group. Moreover, we observed a condition effect (*F*9,288 = 1.401, *p* < 0.001), with a significant decrease of D2 values for the eyes closed compared with the active conditions (Fig. 5). To estimate the condition effect for each group, we performed the Dunnet post hoc test, comparing a single condition (reference) with all the others. We ran 2 separate tests, with the control or patient group values for the eyes closed condition as the reference. In the first test, a significant increase of D2 values was observed (*p* = 0.035 for all but 1 comparison, Fig. 6A), whereas in the second, an increase of D2 values was observed only in the comparison with controls for the eyes closed condition (*p* = 0.035, Fig. 6B). ![Fig. 2](http://jpn.ca/https://www.jpn.ca/content/jpn/37/4/259/F2.medium.gif) [Fig. 2](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/F2) Fig. 2 Global mean of correlation dimension values for each channel and condition in the control group. ![Fig. 3](http://jpn.ca/https://www.jpn.ca/content/jpn/37/4/259/F3.medium.gif) [Fig. 3](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/F3) Fig. 3 Global mean of correlation dimension values for each channel and condition in the patient group. ![Fig. 4](http://jpn.ca/https://www.jpn.ca/content/jpn/37/4/259/F4.medium.gif) [Fig. 4](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/F4) Fig. 4 Analysis of variance results for group effect. A significant increase of mean of correlation dimension values was observed in patients. ![Fig. 5](http://jpn.ca/https://www.jpn.ca/content/jpn/37/4/259/F5.medium.gif) [Fig. 5](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/F5) Fig. 5 Analysis of variance results for condition effect. A significant decrease of mean of correlation dimension values was observed in the eyes closed condition (occipital) compared with the active conditions. C = central; F = frontal; O = occipital; P = parietal. ![Fig. 6](http://jpn.ca/https://www.jpn.ca/content/jpn/37/4/259/F6.medium.gif) [Fig. 6](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/F6) Fig. 6 Analysis of variance results, post hoc Dunnet test. In the (**A**) control group, eyes closed resulted in lower frontal values than the other conditions (*p* < 0.03), whereas in the (**B**) patient group, a significant increase of frontal values was observed only in the comparison with the same condition in the control group (*p* = 0.03). C = central; F = frontal; O = occipital; P = parietal. View this table: [Table 3](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/T3) Table 3 Mean of correlation dimension values for each condition and group by individual electrodes View this table: [Table 4](http://jpn.ca/content/37/4/259/T4) Table 4 Mean of correlation dimension values for each condition and group by region ## Discussion The primary aim of the present study was to investigate D2 in patients with schizophrenia. Recent studies have suggested that noninvasive EEG recordings can yield useful information on pathologic brain functioning. In particular, over the last few years, new nonlinear EEG analysis has been used to detect both pathologic and psychologic/physiologic changes that are not revealed by the use of conventional linear analysis. For example, several studies have reported that physiologic cognitive activities, such as mathematical tasks, induce an increase in D2 levels in healthy controls that is proportional to task complexity.41,42 On the other hand, various studies have shown that patients with Alzheimer disease had significantly lower D2 values than age-matched healthy controls, indicating that the dynamic processes underlying EEG are less complex for patients with Alzheimer disease than for healthy controls.19–21,29 In our study, we investigated patients with schizophrenia by using 2 experimental approaches: first, we assessed the effect of a cognitive task on brain dynamics and, second, we compared brain complexity of patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. The main findings were as follows: first, brain activity was characterized by an increase of D2 values during active conditions (eyes open, counting forward and counting backward conditions) compared with baseline resting state (eyes closed condition) in healthy controls. This increase was not observed in patients with schizophrenia. Moreover, group comparison showed a global enhancement of D2 values in patients compared with controls. Furthermore, we tested the presence of nonlinear dynamics, confirming the prevalence of nonlinearity in the EEGs of healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia. This finding is in agreement with those of previous studies in which nonlinearity of EEGs in patients with schizophrenia was explored11,43,44 and also with those of prior studies showing the presence of nonlinearity in the EEGs of patients with other diseases.28–30 To correctly interpret the results of the present study, the neurophysiologic meaning of D2 has to be discussed. As already mentioned, D2 can be interpreted as a measure of complexity (or flexibility) of information processing and as an index of information integration in the brain. This includes both the integration of the activity of functionally segregated neuronal groups and the integration of incoming stimuli with ongoing, spontaneous brain activity.45 In this light, our results suggest that the increase of information processing and integration that occurred in healthy controls during active conditions and cognitive tasks is not preserved in patients with stable schizophrenia. Impaired information processing is a well-known phenomenon in populations with schizophrenia. Recently, deficits in information processing have been proposed to constitute the endophenotype of schizophrenia. It has been suggested that a breakdown in the processes that regulate the inflow of information from the environment is a core feature of the disease.46 Successful processing of sensory inputs requires the ability to inhibit intrinsic responses to redundant or irrelevant inputs and, reciprocally, to facilitate responses to deviant, novel or salient stimuli. So our result of global increase of brain complexity in the patient group could be explained by this impairment of inhibitory processes. The present results are difficult to compare with those of some studies of patients with schizophrenia owing to differences in experimental conditions, the algorithm chosen to calculate D2,11 sample selection47 and lack of control for surrogate data testing. Whereas a number of studies recruited newly onset, medication-naive and active symptomatic patients with schizophrenia,2,23,48,49 others explored EEG complexity in relatively chronic and less symptomatic patients who were taking medications.10,50,51 In our study, the inclusion criteria were symptom stability for 6 months and pharmacologic treatment with antipsychotic medications. Different studies have addressed the question of whether schizophrenia can be characterized by specific changes in dynamic complexity, but the results appear to be contradictory. Koukkou and colleagues22 found greater D2 values in patients with first-episode schizophrenia than in controls under resting-state and cognitive-task conditions on EEGs of temporoparietal regions. Elbert and colleagues2 reported a higher dynamic complexity during the resting state in patients with schizophrenia in the frontal regions; however, in contrast to Koukkou and colleagues,22 they found a lower dimension at central electrode locations in patients compared with healthy controls. Jeong and colleagues10 found lower D2 values in the left inferior frontal and anterior temporal regions in the EEGs of patients with schizophrenia compared with controls in waking states. Finally, Kirsch and colleagues23 showed that the dimensional complexity was lower in healthy controls than in patients with schizophrenia during complex cognitive tasks. It can be argued that the whole pattern of chaotic dynamics and the topographic distribution of dimensional changes cannot be properly investigated considering the EEG complexity at a limited number of electrodes. Moreover, schizophrenia is a heterogeneous disease, thus a clear and rigorous sample selection is necessary. Our study had some strengths that should be emphasized. First, the patients included in our study were evaluated using established comprehensive tools for the assessment of psychiatric symptoms and neurocognitive functioning. Second, we investigated D2 using an extensive experimental protocol, which allowed us not only to study D2 changes in patients with schizophrenia compared with healthy controls, but also to explore the effect of cognitive activities on physiologic and pathologic brain dynamics. Third, we tested the presence of nonlinearity of our data. Moreover, a recording with an 19-lead EEG allowed us to investigate a more complete pattern of chaotic brain dynamics than studies conducted with a limited number of electrodes (1–16 electrodes in the studies by Elbert and colleagues,2 Jeong and colleagues,10 Koukkou and colleagues22 and Kirsch and colleagues23). ### Limitations Some limitations of the present study should be acknowledged. First, we studied a relatively small sample. Second, all patients with schizophrenia were being treated with anti-psychotic medications, with a mean of 212 (SD 112) CPZ equivalent doses. Although there was no correlation between D2 and antipsychotic treatment, consistent with findings of Itil and colleagues,52 who also reported no effect of medication on EEG activities in patients with schizophrenia, we cannot completely exclude the presence of medication effects. It is tempting to speculate that the observed patterns in patients with schizophrenia might be related to the psychotic processes. However, future studies, preferably conducted in larger patient samples, comparing treated and untreated patients have to be conducted to exclude a possible medication effect. ## Conclusion On the whole, the results of our study suggested that the D2 increase observed in healthy controls when switching from resting to active states, including cognitive tasks, was not preserved in a sample of patients with stable schizophrenia. Moreover, a general increase of brain complexity was observed in the patients with schizophrenia. Those results could be interpreted as an indicator of the information processing deficits that occur in these patients. From a methodologic point of view, our findings suggest that D2 analysis can be used to enrich the set of approaches to analyze EEGs in individuals with pathologic conditions. ## Footnotes * * E. Carlino and M. Sigaudo contributed equally to the work. * **Competing interests:** None declared. * **Contributors:** A. Pollo, F. Benedetti and P. Rocca designed the study. E. Carlino, M. Sigaudo, T. Mongini, F. Castagna and S. Vighetti acquired the data. E. Carlino and M. Sigaudo analyzed the data. E. Carlino, M. Sigaudo and A. Pollo wrote the article. F. Benedetti, T. Mongini, F. Castagna, S. Vighetti and P. Rocca reviewed the article. All authors approved its publication. * Received March 26, 2011. * Revision received October 13, 2011. * Revision received November 17, 2011. * Accepted November 17, 2011. ## References 1. Friston KJ.The disconnection hypothesis.Schizophr Res 1998;30:115–25. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00140-0&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9549774&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000072690100003&link_type=ISI) 2. Elbert T, Lutzenberger W, Rockstroh B, et al.Physical aspects of the EEG in schizophrenics.Biol Psychiatry 1992;32:595–606. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0006-3223(92)90072-8&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=1450286&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1992JZ80500003&link_type=ISI) 3. Itil TM.Qualitative and quantitative EEG findings in schizophrenia.Schizophr Bull 1977;3:61–79. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/schbul/3.1.61&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=325642&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1977DG08500007&link_type=ISI) 4. Pradhan N, Dutt DN.A nonlinear perspective in understanding the neurodynamics of EEG.Comput Biol Med 1993;23:425–42. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8306622&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 5. Babloyantz A, Salazar JM, Nicolis C.Evidence of chaotic dynamics of brain activity during the sleep cycle.Phys Lett A 1985;111:152–6. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0375-9601(85)90444-X&link_type=DOI) 6. Lehnertz K, Elger C.Can epileptic seizures be predicted? Evidence from non-linear time series analysis of brain electrical activity.Phys Rev Lett 1998;80:5019–22. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1103/PhysRevLett.80.5019&link_type=DOI) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000073915100047&link_type=ISI) 7. Pritchard WS, Duke DW, Coburn KL, et al.EEG-based, neural-net predictive classification of Alzheimer’s disease vs. control subjects is augmented by non-linear EEG measures.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994;91:118–30. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0013-4694(94)90033-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7519141&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1994PC37200005&link_type=ISI) 8. Stam KJ, Tavy DLJ, Jelles B.Non-linear dynamical analysis of multi-channel EEG data: clinical applications in dementia and Parkinson’s disease.Brain Topogr 1994;7:141–50. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/BF01186772&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7696091&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 9. Stam CJ, Jelles B, Achtereekte HAM, et al.Diagnostic usefulness of linear and nonlinear quantitative EEG analysis in Alzheimer’s disease.Clin Electroencephalogr 1996;27:69–77. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8681465&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 10. Jeong J, Kim DJ, Chae JH, et al.Non-linear analysis of the EEG of schizophrenics with optimal embedding dimension.Med Eng Phys 1998;20:669–76. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10098611&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 11. Lee YJ, Zhu YS, Xu YH, et al.Detection of non-linearity in the EEG of schizophrenic patients.Clin Neurophysiol 2001;112:1288–94. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00544-2&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11516741&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000169789400016&link_type=ISI) 12. Breakspear M.The nonlinear theory of schizophrenia.Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2006;40:20–35. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1080/j.1440-1614.2006.01737.x&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16403034&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 13. Grassberger P, Procaccia I.Measuring the strangeness of strange attractors.Physica D 1983;9:189–208. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0167-2789(83)90298-1&link_type=DOI) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1983RU46800010&link_type=ISI) 14. Stam CJ.Nonlinear dynamical analysis of EEG and MEG: review of an emerging field.Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:2266–301. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.clinph.2005.06.011&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16115797&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000232233300003&link_type=ISI) 15. Mölle M, Marshall L, Wolf B, et al.EEG complexity and performance measures of creative thinking.Psychophysiology 1999;36:95–104. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1017/S0048577299961619&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10098384&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 16. 1. Markus M, 2. Muller S, 3. Nicolis G Babloyantz A, Destexhe A.The Creutzfeld–Jacob disease in the hierarchy of chaotic attractor.From chemical to biological organizationMarkus M, Muller S, Nicolis GBerlinSpringer1987307–16. 17. Jeong J, Kim DJ, Kim SY, et al.Effect of total sleep deprivation on the dimensional complexity of the waking EEG.Sleep 2001;24:197–202. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11247056&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 18. Babloyantz A, Destexhe A.Low-dimensional chaos in an instance of epilepsy.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1986;83:3513–7. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6NDoicG5hcyI7czo1OiJyZXNpZCI7czoxMDoiODMvMTAvMzUxMyI7czo0OiJhdG9tIjtzOjE4OiIvanBuLzM3LzQvMjU5LmF0b20iO31zOjg6ImZyYWdtZW50IjtzOjA6IiI7fQ==) 19. Besthorn C, Sattel H, Geiger-Kabisch C, et al.Parameters of EEG dimensional complexity in Alzheimer’s disease.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1995;95:84–9. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0013-4694(95)00050-9&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7649009&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1995RR54700003&link_type=ISI) 20. Jeong J, Chae JH, Kim SY, et al.Non-linear dynamical analysis of the EEG in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia.J Clin Neurophysiol 2001;112:827–35. 21. Stam CJ, Jelles B, Achtereekte HAM, et al.Investigation of EEG non-linearity in dementia and Parkinson’s disease.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1995;95:309–17. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0013-4694(95)00147-Q&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7489659&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1995TH96700001&link_type=ISI) 22. Koukkou M, Lehmann D, Wackermann J, et al.Dimensional complexity of EEG brain mechanisms in untreated schizophrenia.Biol Psychiatry 1993;33:397–407. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0006-3223(93)90167-C&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8098223&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1993KZ44200002&link_type=ISI) 23. Kirsch P, Besthorn C, Klein S, et al.The dimensional complexity of the EEG during cognitive tasks reflects the impaired information processing in schizophrenic patients.Int J Psychophysiol 2000;36:237–46. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00077-5&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10754196&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000086755800004&link_type=ISI) 24. Lee SH, Choo JS, Im WY, et al.Nonlinear analysis of electroencephalogram in schizophrenia patients with persistent auditory hallucination.Psychiatry Investig 2008;5:115–20. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=20046354&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 25. Theiler J, Eubank S, Longtin A, et al.Testing for nonlinearity in time series: the method of surrogate data.Physica D 1992;58:77–94. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0167-2789(92)90102-S&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1992JV85800&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1992JV85800006&link_type=ISI) 26. Prichard D, Theiler J.Generating surrogate data for time series with several simultaneously measured variables.Phys Rev Lett 1994;73:951–4. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1103/PhysRevLett.73.951&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10057582&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1994PB98800010&link_type=ISI) 27. Shen Y, Olbrich E, Achermann P, et al.Dimensional complexity and spectral properties of the human sleep EEG. Electroencephalograms.Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:199–209. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00338-3&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12559226&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000181094000004&link_type=ISI) 28. Jung KY, Kim JM, Kim DW.Nonlinear dynamic characteristics of electroencephalography in a high-dose pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus model.Epilepsy Res 2003;54:179–88. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12837569&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 29. Jelles B, Strijers RL, Hooijer C, et al.Nonlinear EEG analysis in early Alzheimer’s disease.Acta Neurol Scand 1999;100:360–8. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10589795&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000083701200002&link_type=ISI) 30. Stam CJ, Jelles B, Achtereekte HA, et al.Investigation of EEG nonlinearity in dementia and Parkinson’s disease.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1995;95:309–17. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0013-4694(95)00147-Q&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=7489659&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1995TH96700001&link_type=ISI) 31. American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (The Association, Washington), 4th ed, text revised. 32. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, et al. (1997) Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV disorders (SCID) (American Psychiatric Press, Washington (DC)). 33. Guy W (1976) ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology (US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute of Mental Health, Psychopharmacology Research Branch, Division of Extramural Research Programs, Rockville (MD)). 34. Delis DC, Kramer J, Kaplan E, et al. (1987) California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) manual (Psychological Corporation, San Antonio (TX)). 35. Jones SH, Thornicroft G, Coffey M, et al.A brief mental health out- come scale-reliability and validity of the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF).Br J Psychiatry 1995;166:654–9. [Abstract/FREE Full Text](http://jpn.ca/lookup/ijlink/YTozOntzOjQ6InBhdGgiO3M6MTQ6Ii9sb29rdXAvaWpsaW5rIjtzOjU6InF1ZXJ5IjthOjQ6e3M6ODoibGlua1R5cGUiO3M6NDoiQUJTVCI7czoxMToiam91cm5hbENvZGUiO3M6MTA6ImJqcHJjcHN5Y2giO3M6NToicmVzaWQiO3M6OToiMTY2LzUvNjU0IjtzOjQ6ImF0b20iO3M6MTg6Ii9qcG4vMzcvNC8yNTkuYXRvbSI7fXM6ODoiZnJhZ21lbnQiO3M6MDoiIjt9) 36. Kay SR, Opler LA, Fiszbein A.The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.Schizophr Bull 1987;13:261–76. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/schbul/13.2.261&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=3616518&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1987H977500007&link_type=ISI) 37. Stroop JR.Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.J Exp Psychol 1935;18:643–62. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1037/h0054651&link_type=DOI) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000188522600055&link_type=ISI) 38. Reitan R.Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indication of organic brain damage.Percept Mot Skills 1958;8:271–6. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271&link_type=DOI) 39. Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, et al. (1993) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual (Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc, Odessa (FL)). 40. Pereda E, Quiroga RQ, Bhattacharya J.Nonlinear multivariate analysis of neurophysiological signals.Prog Neurobiol 2005;77:1–37. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.10.003&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=16289760&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000234055700001&link_type=ISI) 41. Gregson RA, Campbell EA, Gates GR.Cognitive load as a determinant of the dimensionality of the electroencephalogram: a replication study.Biol Psychol 1993;35:165–78. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/0301-0511(93)90012-W&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8507745&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 42. Xu N, Xu JH.The fractal dimension of EEG as a physical measure of conscious human brain activities.Bull Math Biol 1988;50:559–65. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/BF02458853&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=3233420&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1988Q716500009&link_type=ISI) 43. Breakspear M, Terry J, Friston K, et al.A disturbance of nonlinear interdependence in scalp EEG of subjects with first episode schizophrenia.Neuroimage 2003;20:466–78. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00332-X&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=14527607&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000185746400043&link_type=ISI) 44. Rubinov M, Knock SA, Stam CJ, et al.Small-world properties of nonlinear brain activity in schizophrenia.Hum Brain Mapp 2009;30:403–16. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1002/hbm.20517&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=18072237&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000263232800006&link_type=ISI) 45. Tononi G, Edelman GM.Consciousness and the integration of information in the brain.Adv Neurol 1998;77:245–79. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9709829&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 46. Turetsky BI, Calkins ME, Light GA, et al.Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected candidate measures.Schizophr Bull 2007;33:69–94. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1093/schbul/sbl060&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=17135482&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000243070900009&link_type=ISI) 47. Kang UG, Par KT, Ahn YM, et al.Non-linear dynamic analysis of clozapine-induced electroencephalographic changes in schizophrenic patients — a preliminary study.Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2001;25:1229–39. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0278-5846(01)00183-X&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=11474842&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 48. Koukkou M, Lehmann D, Federspiel A, et al.EEG reactivity and EEG activity in never-treated acute schizophrenics, measured with spectral parameters and dimensional complexity.J Neural Transm Gen Sect 1995;99:89–102. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/BF01271472&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=8579811&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) 49. Saito N, Kuginuki T, Yagyu T, et al.Global, regional, and local measures of complexity of multichannel electroencephalography in acute, neuroleptic-naive, first-break schizophrenics.Biol Psychiatry 1998;43:794–802. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00547-7&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9611668&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000073738900003&link_type=ISI) 50. Hoffmann RE, Buchsbaum MS, Jensen RV, et al.Dimensional complexity of EEG waveforms in neuroleptic-free schizophrenic patients and normal control subjects.J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1996;8:436–41. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9116481&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1996VQ81600010&link_type=ISI) 51. Jin SH, Na SH, Kim SY, et al.Hemispheric laterality and dimensional complexity in schizophrenia under sound and light stimulation.Int J Psychophysiol 2003;49:1–15. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00078-3&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=12853126&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=000184419400001&link_type=ISI) 52. Itil TM, Saletu B, Davis S.EEG findings in chronic schizophrenics based on digital computer period analysis and analog power spectra.Biol Psychiatry 1972;5:1–13. [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=5042313&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1972N007600001&link_type=ISI) 53. Eckmann JP, Ruelle D.Ergodic theory of chaos and strange attractors.Rev Mod Phys 1985;57:617–56. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1103/RevModPhys.57.617&link_type=DOI) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1985APC5700001&link_type=ISI) 54. Takens F.Detecting strange attractors in turbulence in dynamical systems and turbulence.Lecture Notes in Math 1981;898:366–81. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1007/BFb0091924&link_type=DOI) 55. Kennel MB, Brown R, Abarbanel HDI.Determining embedding dimension for phase-space reconstruction using a geometrical construction.Phys Rev A 1992;45:3403–11. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1103/PhysRevA.45.3403&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=9907388&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom) [Web of Science](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=A1992HK38400010&link_type=ISI) 56. Hornero R, Alonso A, Jimeno N, et al.Nonlinear analysis of time series generated by schizophrenic patients.IEEE Eng Med Biol Mag 1999;18:84–90. [CrossRef](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10.1109/51.765193&link_type=DOI) [PubMed](http://jpn.ca/lookup/external-ref?access_num=10337567&link_type=MED&atom=%2Fjpn%2F37%2F4%2F259.atom)