Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Issues
    • Issue in progress
    • Issues by date
  • Sections
    • Editorial
    • Review
    • Research
    • Commentary
    • Psychopharmacology for the Clinician
    • Letters to the Editor
  • Topic Collections
  • Instructions for Authors
    • Overview for authors
    • Submission checklist
    • Editorial policies
    • Publication fees
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Dr. Francis Wayne Quan Memorial Prize
    • Open access
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • About
    • General information
    • Staff
    • Editorial Board
    • Contact
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ
    • CMAJ Open
    • CJS
    • JAMC

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
JPN
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ
    • CMAJ Open
    • CJS
    • JAMC
JPN

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Issues
    • Issue in progress
    • Issues by date
  • Sections
    • Editorial
    • Review
    • Research
    • Commentary
    • Psychopharmacology for the Clinician
    • Letters to the Editor
  • Topic Collections
  • Instructions for Authors
    • Overview for authors
    • Submission checklist
    • Editorial policies
    • Publication fees
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Dr. Francis Wayne Quan Memorial Prize
    • Open access
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • About
    • General information
    • Staff
    • Editorial Board
    • Contact
  • Subscribe to our alerts
  • RSS feeds
  • Follow JPN on Twitter
Research Papers

Impaired musical ability in people with schizophrenia

Sanae Hatada, Ken Sawada, Masanori Akamatsu, Erina Doi, Masayoshi Minese, Motoshi Yamashita, Allen E. Thornton, William G. Honer and Shimpei Inoue
J Psychiatry Neurosci March 01, 2014 39 (2) 118-126; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.120207
Sanae Hatada
Hatada, Sawada, Akamatsu, Inoue — Department of Neuropsychiatry, Aizu Medical Center, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan; Sawada, Honer — Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada; Sawada, Doi, Minese — Kochi Prefectural Aki General Hospital, Kochi, Japan; Yamashita — Kochi Health Sciences Center, Kochi, Japan; Thornton — Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ken Sawada
Hatada, Sawada, Akamatsu, Inoue — Department of Neuropsychiatry, Aizu Medical Center, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan; Sawada, Honer — Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada; Sawada, Doi, Minese — Kochi Prefectural Aki General Hospital, Kochi, Japan; Yamashita — Kochi Health Sciences Center, Kochi, Japan; Thornton — Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: [email protected]
Masanori Akamatsu
Hatada, Sawada, Akamatsu, Inoue — Department of Neuropsychiatry, Aizu Medical Center, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan; Sawada, Honer — Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada; Sawada, Doi, Minese — Kochi Prefectural Aki General Hospital, Kochi, Japan; Yamashita — Kochi Health Sciences Center, Kochi, Japan; Thornton — Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Erina Doi
Hatada, Sawada, Akamatsu, Inoue — Department of Neuropsychiatry, Aizu Medical Center, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan; Sawada, Honer — Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada; Sawada, Doi, Minese — Kochi Prefectural Aki General Hospital, Kochi, Japan; Yamashita — Kochi Health Sciences Center, Kochi, Japan; Thornton — Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Masayoshi Minese
Hatada, Sawada, Akamatsu, Inoue — Department of Neuropsychiatry, Aizu Medical Center, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan; Sawada, Honer — Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada; Sawada, Doi, Minese — Kochi Prefectural Aki General Hospital, Kochi, Japan; Yamashita — Kochi Health Sciences Center, Kochi, Japan; Thornton — Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Motoshi Yamashita
Hatada, Sawada, Akamatsu, Inoue — Department of Neuropsychiatry, Aizu Medical Center, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan; Sawada, Honer — Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada; Sawada, Doi, Minese — Kochi Prefectural Aki General Hospital, Kochi, Japan; Yamashita — Kochi Health Sciences Center, Kochi, Japan; Thornton — Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Allen E. Thornton
Hatada, Sawada, Akamatsu, Inoue — Department of Neuropsychiatry, Aizu Medical Center, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan; Sawada, Honer — Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada; Sawada, Doi, Minese — Kochi Prefectural Aki General Hospital, Kochi, Japan; Yamashita — Kochi Health Sciences Center, Kochi, Japan; Thornton — Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
William G. Honer
Hatada, Sawada, Akamatsu, Inoue — Department of Neuropsychiatry, Aizu Medical Center, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan; Sawada, Honer — Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada; Sawada, Doi, Minese — Kochi Prefectural Aki General Hospital, Kochi, Japan; Yamashita — Kochi Health Sciences Center, Kochi, Japan; Thornton — Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Shimpei Inoue
Hatada, Sawada, Akamatsu, Inoue — Department of Neuropsychiatry, Aizu Medical Center, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan; Sawada, Honer — Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada; Sawada, Doi, Minese — Kochi Prefectural Aki General Hospital, Kochi, Japan; Yamashita — Kochi Health Sciences Center, Kochi, Japan; Thornton — Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Tables
  • Related Content
  • Responses
  • Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

This article has a correction. Please see:

  • Correction - May 01, 2015

Abstract

Background: Assessment of the musical ability of people with schizophrenia has attracted little interest despite the diverse and substantive findings of impairments in sound perception and processing and the therapeutic effect of music in people with the illness. The present study investigated the musical ability of people with schizophrenia and the association with psychiatric symptoms and cognition.

Methods: We recruited patients with chronic schizophrenia and healthy controls for participation in our study. To measure musical ability and cognitive function, we used the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA) and the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS). We carried out a mediation analysis to investigate a possible pathway to a deficit in musical ability.

Results: We enrolled 50 patients and 58 controls in the study. The MBEA global score in patients with schizophrenia was significantly lower than that in controls (p < 0.001), and was strongly associated with both the composite cognitive function score (r = 0.645, p < 0.001) and the negative symptom score (r = −0.504, p < 0.001). Further analyses revealed direct and indirect effects of negative symptoms on musical ability. The indirect effects were mediated through cognitive impairment.

Limitations: The relatively small sample size did not permit full evaluation of the possible effects of age, sex, education, medication and cultural influences on the results.

Conclusion: Examining the associations between musical deficits, negative symptoms and cognitive imapirment in patients with schizophrenia may identify shared biological mechanisms.

Introduction

Like language, music is a universal, human-specific cultural experience. Perception, interpretation and production of music are linked to language, emotion, cognitive function and social communication in everyday life. Neurocognitive and clinical studies have investigated musical emotion, cognition and musical disorders.1,2 Deficits in musical processing, or amusia, have been intensely investigated for more than a decade.3 Amusia can be identified by the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA), used as a standard battery of tests for music assessment in people with acquired (secondary to brain injury or illness) or congenital (“tone deafness,” detectable early in childhood) forms of the disorder.4,5

Music perception and processing in healthy individuals are influenced by several factors, including sex, age, musical experience and cultural background. Sex differences have been observed in both children and adults when measuring early right anterior electrophysiological activity in processing musical information.6 Maturation of the brain through childhood, adolescence and early adulthood, and aging of the brain in elderly people have a wide range of effects on musical ability and on activation of specific brain regions while testing musical ability.7–9 Active musical training has distinct effects from those of maturation or aging.9 Culture and language effects can influence some parts of the MBEA, such as the metre test, and are also reported to alter the sensitivity of pitch perception.10,11

The association between musical ability and cognitive function has been investigated in patients with neurologic illnesses. In those with Alzheimer disease, musical performance is preserved to a greater degree than other mental activities and is relatively preserved even in the most severe stages of illness.12–14 Recent findings indicate middle cerebral artery stroke can cause acquired amusia,15–17 which is associated with auditory cortex and frontal lobe damage, with the expected patterns of cognitive impairment following damage to these regions.15 Three months after a stroke, improvement in musical deficit is linked to recovery of verbal learning, visuospatial perception and attention, suggesting that musical ability in people with acquired amusia is associated with multiple cognitive functions.15,16 In contrast to the large body of literature on the association of neurologic disorders and cognitive impairment with musical ability,18 less is known about the effects of severe mental illnesses.

Schizophrenia is a major mental disorder that manifests several core features, including positive symptoms, such as hallucinations and delusions, and negative symptoms, such as apathy and poverty of speech.19 In addition to clinically obvious psychiatric symptoms, schizophrenia is frequently complicated by neuropsychological or cognitive impairment, which can be as severe as clinical dementia.20 While the positive symptoms of schizophrenia show little association with cognitive impairment, the negative symptoms demonstrate a complex association with cognition in the illness.21,22 In most studies, negative symptoms and cognitive impairment covary, but these domains of illness appear to contribute to a distinct and interrelated pathway in predicting real-world functional impairment.21,23 However, negative symptoms are also present in the rare cases of schizophrenia where cognition is preserved at a superior level.24 Both symptoms and cognitive impairment in patients with schizophrenia may be related to abnormal sensory processing mechanisms, several of which are relevant to music and the brain.25 These include auditory event-related evoked potentials, such as P50, P300 and mismatch negativity.26–28 Dysfunction of auditory information processing and altered perception of pitch and intensity are also observed.29 Abnormalities in detection of the prosodic qualities of speech in patients with schizophrenia are linked to auditory pathways.30 The negative symptoms of schizophrenia are also associated with abnormalities in detecting frequency modulated tones designed to mimic key acoustic features of human vocal expressions.31

Exploration of the musical abilities of patients with schizophrenia may provide a unique opportunity to link the symptoms of illness to an important aspect of brain function. In this study, we used the MBEA, which is widely used for assessment of musical ability, to determine whether patients with schizophrenia have a deficit in musical ability. We examined associations between musical ability and sex, age, musical training, education and the characteristic symptomatology of schizophrenia. Finally, we carried out an exploratory mediation analysis to investigate the pathway from the symptoms of schizophrenia to musical disability. We were specifically interested in the possible role of a causal pathway from negative symptoms, mediated through cognitive impairments described in patients with schizophrenia, to music disability.32

Methods

Participants

We recruited patients with schizophrenia from 3 mental hospitals in the southwest of Japan. The patients had either schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, which was diagnosed according to ICD-10. Exclusion criteria were history of neurologic disorder and current substance abuse or dependence. We also recruited healthy controls from the community via advertisement. We obtained written informed consent from all participants after the study was described to them. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Kochi Medical School.

Measurement of musical ability

We used the MBEA to measure the musical ability of patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. The MBEA is composed of 6 tests or subscales: Scale, Contour, Interval, Rhythm, Meter and Memory. Scale, Contour and Interval measure melodic ability, while Rhythm and Meter measure temporal ability. Each test uses musical phases designed to test specific abilities. The mean duration of each musical phrase is 5.1 (range 3.8–6.4) seconds, with the exception of the metric test in which phrases are twice as long. The phrases are presented with a piano sound. For examples and additional detail, see the Peretz laboratory website (www.brams.umontreal.ca). For the subscales of Scale, Contour, Interval and Rhythm, we presented 2 musical phrases in succession to the participant, who was then asked if the phrases were the same or different. For the Meter subscale, the participant was asked if the phrase was a march or a waltz. For the Memory subscale, the participant was asked if a presented phrase had already been presented. We presented participants with 30 questions for each subscale. All 6 sub-scale sessions were completed in approximately 1 hour, and we used the mean for the 6 scale scores as the global score. Chance level was 50% for all tests. The interviewer (S.H.) encouraged participants to respond to the tests, and good cooperation was obtained from all participants.

Assessment of mental state and cognitive function

To evaluate the symptoms of schizophrenia, patients were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), the Clinical Global Impression scales and the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) scale.33,34 To assess possible extrapyramidal motor side effects of antipsychotic medications, we used the overall severity score of the Drug-Induced Extrapyramidal Symptoms Scale (DIEPSS), including assessment of dyskinesia, akathisia, dystonia and Parkinsonism. Overall severity on the DIEPSS ranges from zero (normal) to 4 (severe).35 To examine cognitive functions, we used the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia; Japanese version (BACS).32

Statistical analysis

We evaluated data for normality using the Shapiro–Wilks test. Possible differences in demographic variables between patients and controls were assessed with Student t test for normally distributed variables or the Mann–Whitney U non-parametric test, as appropriate. We used similar approaches to test for differences in musical ability between patients and controls and between patients with normal musical ability and those with amusia. We set the amusia cut-off score as lower than 2 standard deviations (SDs) below the control mean MBEA global score (equating to 76.8% correct), which is very similar to the cut-off for amusia of 77.6% reported in the original description of the MBEA.5 We used partial correlations to examine the association between global musical score and clinical variables and cognitive functions, controlling for age and sex. For musical training, we used the Kendall τ statistic, and we used Pearson correlation coefficients for all other correlations. All analyses were performed using SPSS version 11 (SPSS Japan).

We conducted mediation analyses using the SPSS macro Indirect, as described by Preacher and Hayes.36 A mediation analysis is a test of a causal model, describing the extent to which an independent variable affects an outcome variable, through an intervening variable (a mediator).36,37 A logical ordering of the independent variable, mediator and outcome variable can often be specified based on their temporal ordering. In the present cross-sectional study of schizophrenia, establishing temporal ordering for the putative independent (symptoms) and mediator (cognition) variables was not possible. Because cognition is proximal to musical ability in various clinical conditions, we evaluated cognition as the mediator to account for the link between symptoms and musical abilities. The proposed mediation model is considered to be the most parsimonious (see the Introduction), but is not the only model that could be evaluated.38

Using bootstrapped standard errors, mediation is established if the 95% bias-corrected and accelerated confidence interval for the indirect effect does not include zero. We first examined total symptom severity to assess associations between total symptoms, cognitive function and musical ability. We then investigated the specificity of these findings to positive or negative symptoms. To estimate the effects of positive versus negative symptoms within the context of the potential intervening role of cognition (indirect effects), we conducted 2 multiple independent mediation analyses. In the first, positive symptom severity served as the independent variable, with the negative symptom score and age as covariates. For the second analysis, negative symptom severity was the independent variable, with the positive symptom score and age as covariates. In both analyses the direct effects of the symptom scores and the indirect effects of the cognitive mediator were established. Mediation analyses were controlled for possible effects of age.

Results

Participants

All participants were ethnic Japanese from Kochi Prefecture in the southwest of Japan. We enrolled 50 patients with schizophrenia and 58 controls in the study. All patients were treated with antipsychotics. The demographic and clinical characteristics of participants are summarized in Table 1. There was no significant difference in age or years of musical training (typically during childhood, ages 4–10 yr, most commonly a keyboard instrument, such as a piano) between the groups, but patients with schizophrenia had significantly fewer years of general education than controls. Twenty-two of 58 controls and 14 of 50 patients with schizophrenia had amateur musical training. As expected, the patient group showed significant cognitive impairment compared with the healthy control group (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1

Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) composite and subscale scores. *p < 0.01, Student t test.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Demographics and clinical characteristics of patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls

MBEA scores

The MBEA global mean score differed between the groups (71.1% for patients with schizophrenia v. 88.4% for controls, t65 = 8.57, p < 0.001; Fig. 2A), and showed a normal distribution in each group, as reported elsewhere.5 In addition, for all 6 MBEA subscales shown in Fig. 2A, the patient group had lower scores than the control group (all p < 0.001). Rhythm, Meter and Memory scores were not normally distributed in the control group (likely owing to ceiling effects); all scores in the musical subcategories were normally distributed in the schizophrenia group. Three of 58 (5.2%) controls and 31 of 50 (62%) patients with schizophrenia satisfied the amusia criteria. Patients with and without amusia, respectively, differed in age (mean 41.8 ± 8.6 yr v. 54.3 ± 8.6 yr, t48 = 4.95, p < 0.001), years of education (mean 11.5 ± 1.9 yr v. 13.2 ± 2.4 yr, t48 = −2.79, p = 0.008) and duration of illness (mean 26.6 ± 9.1 yr v. 18.6 ± 11.4 yr, t48 = 2.75, p = 0.008).

Fig. 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2

(A) Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA) global score and its subscales, (B) MBEA global score and sex, and MBEA global score with (C) age, (D) years of education and (E) years of musical training. Dashed regression lines in panels C and D refer to controls, and solid lines refer to patients with schizophrenia. C = controls; S = patients with schizophrenia. *p < 0.05.

Correlations with MBEA global score

To identify possible variables linked to musical disability, we analyzed the effect of sex on musical ability and the correlation of the MBEA global score with age, education and musical training, as shown in Figure 2B–E and Table 2. The MBEA global score for women was higher than that for men in both the control group (90.0% v. 86.8%, t56 = 2.18, p = 0.033) and the schizophrenia group (75.8% v. 68.0%, t48 = 2.14, p = 0.038; Fig. 2B). The global MBEA score was significantly associated with age in both controls and patients with schizophrenia (Table 2 and Fig. 2C), with years of education only in patients with schizophrenia (Table 2 and Fig. 2D), and with years of musical training in both controls and patients with schizophrenia (Table 2 and Fig. 2E).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

Association of Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia global score with demographic and clinical characteristics

Association of clinical features of illness and severity of symptoms with MBEA global score

We first examined the effect of clinical features of illness and severity of symptoms on MBEA global score using partial correlation analyses. These were followed by analyses related to cognitive function. In the mediation analysis, we considered both symptom severity and cognitive function. Lower negative and general psychopathology symptom scores on the PANSS and higher scores on the GAF (both indicating milder psychopathology) were associated with higher MBEA global scores (Table 2 and Fig. 3A). Other correlations between clinical variables and the MBEA global score were not significant. Of note, antipsychotic medications are more effective in treating positive rather than negative symptoms of schizophrenia, which is consistent with the lack of association between medication dose and MBEA global score.

Fig. 3
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 3

(A) Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA) global score and Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale subscale scores in schizophrenia, and (B) MBEA global score and the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) composite and subscale scores in patients with schizophrenia.

Association of musical ability with cognitive function

Statistical analysis and scatter plots demonstrating associations between cognitive impairment and MBEA global score in the patient group are shown in Table 2 and Figure 3B. A partial correlation study of the schizophrenia group showed a significant association between MBEA global score and the composite score of the BACS. In the subtests of patients with schizophrenia, there were significant associations between musical score and all cognitive functions. As seen in the Appendix, Table S1, available at cma.ca/jpn, symptom severity was also associated with cognitive impairment.

Direct and indirect effects of psychopathology on musical ability in patients with schizophrenia

To evaluate the associations between global symptom severity, specific symptom types, cognitive function and musical deficits, we performed mediation analyses using 3 models (Appendix, Figure S1, and Table 3). Model 1 employed the PANSS total score as the independent variable, the MBEA global score as the outcome and the composite BACS score as the mediation variable, whereas models 2 and 3 used the PANSS positive and negative symptoms scores as multiple independent variables.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3

Bootstrap results for indirect effects of symptom severity on the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia global score via the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia composite score in 50 patients with schizophrenia, controlled for age and sex

In model 1 (Appendix, Fig. S1, and Table 3), mediation analysis based on 10 000 bootstrapped samples showed that after controlling for age and sex, the PANSS total score had an indirect effect operating through the BACS composite score (ab paths). The indirect effect accounted for approximately 48% (−0.145 of −0.302) of the total effect.

Next, in models 2 and 3, we assessed the extent to which cognitive function mediated the effects that positive and negative symptoms have on musical ability. In model 2, the PANSS positive symptom score had no significant direct or indirect effect on musical ability after controlling for the negative symptom score, age and sex. In contrast, in model 3, the PANSS negative symptoms score had both a significant direct effect on musical ability and an indirect effect on musical ability that was mediated by the BACS composite score after controlling for the effect of positive symptoms, age and sex. The indirect effect accounted for about 43% (−0.445 ÷ −1.030) of the total effect.

Discussion

The MBEA global score was lower in patients with schizophrenia than in controls. Clinical features of schizophrenia associated with poorer musical ability were higher severity of symptoms and lower global functioning. In patients with schizophrenia, greater cognitive impairment was associated with poorer musical ability. Mediation analyses showed that negative symptoms in patients with schizophrenia have both direct and indirect effects on musical disability, with the indirect effect mediated through cognitive impairment.

We found a markedly lower mean MBEA global score in the schizophrenia group than in the control group, and 62% of patients with schizophrenia were categorized as having amusia compared with 5% of control participants. The latter finding is consistent with a reported 3%–4% estimate of amusia in Western samples.39,40 There are 2 interpretations of this difference. One possibility is that onset and progression of schizophrenia causes musical disability (acquired amusia). The other is that some patients with schizophrenia may have had congenital amusia or “tone deafness,” perhaps related to a brain mechanism that also predisposes to schizophrenia (although this remains speculative). Since we did not determine history of tone deafness when interviewing participants, we were not able to evaluate whether the musical deficits were due to acquired or congenital amusia. Family, premorbid and first-episode studies would be useful in clarifying when the musical deficits can be first observed and to what extent the deficits increase during the course of the illness.

Acoustic studies of patients with schizophrenia have demonstrated impaired processing of sound intensity changes,29 and difficulties in extracting emotional meaning from prosody.41,42 Verbal prosodic pitch discrimination has also been reported to be impaired and to correlate with positive symptoms.43 Neuropsychological studies of patients with specific lesions suggest musical processing requires different and higher levels of cognitive function than sound processing.44 Emotions experienced when listening to music are closely related to reward mechanisms through a network of mesolimbic structures, such as the nucleus accumbens and ventral tegmental area.45 These mechanisms involve the neurotransmitter dopamine, which is central to the pathophysiology and treatment of schizophrenia.46,47 Attention can also be drawn to the hippocampus and anterior cingulate cortex. These regions participate in the emotional response to music in health and show altered synaptic mechanisms related to brain plasticity in patients with schizophrenia.48–50

Positive symptoms, negative symptoms and cognitive impairment are core features of schizophrenia.19 In our study, negative symptoms were correlated with the MBEA global score. Patients with schizophrenia also showed a correlation between overall cognitive functions and global musical score. Our mediation study showed an approximately 43% indirect and 57% direct association between negative symptoms and musical deficit via cognitive impairment, indicating that the effects of negative symptoms on musical disability in patients with schizophrenia are only partially explained by cognitive impairment. This is consistent with reports indicating that negative symptoms and cognitive impairment are strongly correlated, but that they also show discriminative features.22,51–53 Meta-analysis showed that participation in music therapy (7–78 sessions at a frequency of 1–6 sessions per wk for 1–4 mo) resulted in an improvement in negative symptoms, general mental state and some aspects of cognitive function in patients with schizophrenia.54 The association between improvement in symptoms, cognition and possibly in musical ability is not clear.55 Of interest, poststroke studies of indivdiuals with acquired amusia have shown that improvement of verbal memory, attention and executive function was correlated with musical recovery.15,16 Neural plasticity may be the key to rehabilitation of patients with schizophrenia and poststroke and could play a role in recovery of traditionally described cognitive functions as well as musical ability.56 In addition to antipsychotic pharmacotherapy, innovative treatment approaches ranging from music therapy to cognitive remediation to aerobic exercise may represent the application of neuroscience to rehabilitation of neural plasticity in patients with schizophrenia.57–59

Limitations

Since the MBEA was initially established for populations with a Western musical background, cultural differences in music might affect its reliability.10,11 The MBEA global score for controls in this study was comparable with results obtained in Canadian, Chinese and Greek populations.1,4,10 A larger sample size would have allowed us to perform a more detailed comparison of the MBEA subscale results in our homogeneous Japanese sample to results in other racial and cultural groups reported in the literature. The MBEA global score appears to be an effective screening tool for amusia in the Japanese population, but may miss more subtle or domain-specific forms of amusia. Given the age of our participants, it is highly likely that they were all exposed to Western music, even when growing up. More detailed understanding of cultural influences on learning music could have implications for understanding musical abilities in people with mental illness.

The patient and control groups differed in mean level of general education by 2.7 years. Although the shorter duration of education in the patient group was likely associated with the onset of illness, and the extent of musical training did not differ between the groups, lower level of education could contribute to lower MBEA scores in the schizophrenia group. As noted previously, family, premorbid and first-episode studies would help clarify the possible effects of education (both general and musical) on the deficits in musical ability that we observed. Although we observed differences in MBEA global score related to sex as well as correlations with age, the relatively small sample size precluded detailed estimation of these effects over a broad age range or possible interactions. We cannot exclude the possibility that musical training could influence the results, even though these potential confounds did not differ between the groups. A larger sample size would also allow consideration of associations between subscale findings from the MBEA and clinical features of schizophrenia. Identifying dissociations between specific aspects of cognitive impairment and specific musical abilities would provide an opportunity to strengthen the mediation analysis findings with a complementary strategy. Finally, although no correlation was observed between musical ability and antipsychotic medication dose in our study, a larger study with a wider range of medication types and doses is needed to thoroughly examine the possible effects of medication on MBEA scores.

Conclusion

Mean scores in a test of global musical ability were lower than expected in a group of patients with schizophrenia, and most patients were classified as demonstrating amusia. Impaired musical ability was linked to clinical features of illness. Examination of a potential pathway to the deficit identified direct effects of negative symptoms and indirect effects mediated through cognitive impairment. Musical ability may be a unique human attribute and schizophrenia a unique human ailment. Greater understanding of the connection between amusia and schizophrenia could improve our understanding of the neural substrates of functional impairment in people with this illness.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for the cooperation of all study participants and the assistance of Dr. Yasuhiko Sudo, Dr. Yoichiro Takasaka, and all the staff of Tosa Hospital, Hosogi Unity Hospital and Aki General Hospital. This work was supported by the Kochi Medical School Alumni Association.

Footnotes

  • Competing interests: A.E. Thornton has a grant from the Mind Foundation of the British Columbia Schizophrenia Society and has received consulting fees from the Provincial Health Services Authority. W.G. Honer has a grant from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research and has received consulting fees from MDH Consulting, Novartis, Roche, Lundbeck, Rush University, the Korean Society for Schizophrenia Research, the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, the British Columbia Schizophrenia Society, the Fraser Health Authority, the Vancouver Coastal Health Authority, the Provincial Health Services Authority and the Canadian Agency for Drugs and Technology in Health. He is also on the board of In Silico Biosciences. No other competing interests declared.

  • Contributors: K. Sawada designed the study. S. Hatada, K. Sawada, M. Akamatsu, E. Doi, M. Minese and M. Ramashita acquired the data, which K. Sawada, A.E. Thornton, W.G. Honer and S. Inoue analyzed. S. Hatada, K. Sawada, W.G. Honer and S. Inoue wrote the article, which all authors reviewed and approved for publications.

  • Received October 22, 2012.
  • Revision received April 15, 2013.
  • Revision received June 12, 2013.
  • Revision received June 18, 2013.
  • Accepted June 25, 2013.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Nan Y,
    2. Sun Y,
    3. Peretz I
    .Congenital amusia in speakers of a tone language: association with lexical tone agnosia.Brain 2010;133:2635–42.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Zatorre R,
    2. McGill J
    .Music, the food of neuroscience?.Nature 2005;434:312–5.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Levitin DJ,
    2. Tirovolas AK
    .Current advances in the cognitive neuroscience of music.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1156:211–31.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Peretz I,
    2. Ayotte J,
    3. Zatorre RJ,
    4. et al
    .Congenital amusia: a disorder of fine-grained pitch discrimination.Neuron 2002;33:185–91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Peretz I,
    2. Champod AS,
    3. Hyde K
    .Varieties of musical disorders: the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003;999:58–75.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Koelsch S,
    2. Grossmann T,
    3. Gunter TC,
    4. et al
    .Children processing music: electric brain responses reveal musical competence and gender differences.J Cogn Neurosci 2003;15:683–93.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Halpern AR,
    2. Bartlett JC,
    3. Dowling WJ
    .Perception of mode, rhythm, and contour in unfamiliar melodies: effects of age and experience.Music Percept 1998;15:335–55.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Halpern AR,
    2. Bartlett JC
    .Aging and memory for music: a review.Psychomusicology: music, mind, and brain 2002;18:10–27.
    OpenUrl
  8. ↵
    1. Ellis RJ,
    2. Norton AC,
    3. Overy K,
    4. et al
    .Differentiating maturational and training influences on fMRI activation during music processing.Neuroimage 2012;60:1902–12.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Paraskevopoulos E,
    2. Tsapkini K,
    3. Peretz I
    .Cultural aspects of music perception: validation of a Greek version of the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusias.J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:695–704.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Wong PC,
    2. Ciocca V,
    3. Chan AH,
    4. et al
    .Effects of culture on musical pitch perception.PLoS ONE 2012;7:e33424
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Crystal HA,
    2. Grober E,
    3. Masur D
    .Preservation of musical memory in Alzheimer’s disease.J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1989;52:1415–6.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
    1. Cuddy LL,
    2. Duffin J
    .Music, memory, and Alzheimer’s disease: Is music recognition spared in dementia, and how can it be assessed?.Med Hypotheses 2005;64:229–35.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Vanstone AD,
    2. Cuddy LL,
    3. Duffin JM,
    4. et al
    .Exceptional preservation of memory for tunes and lyrics: case studies of amusia, profound deafness, and Alzheimer’s disease.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1169:291–4.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Särkämö T,
    2. Tervaniemi M,
    3. Soinila S,
    4. et al
    .Cognitive deficits associated with acquired amusia after stroke: a neuropsychological follow-up study.Neuropsychologia 2009;47:2642–51.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Särkämö T,
    2. Tervaniemi M,
    3. Soinila S,
    4. et al
    .Auditory and cognitive deficits associated with acquired amusia after stroke: a magnetoencephalography and neuropsychological follow-up study.PLoS ONE 2010;5:e15157
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Särkämö T,
    2. Pihko E,
    3. Laitinen S,
    4. et al
    .Music and speech listening enhance the recovery of early sensory processing after stroke.J Cogn Neurosci 2010;22:2716–27.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Brust JC
    .Music and the neurologist: a historical perspective.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2001;930:143–52.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Schultz SK,
    2. Andreasen NC
    .Schizophrenia.Lancet 1999;353:1425–30.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. de Vries PJ,
    2. Honer WG,
    3. Kemp PM,
    4. et al
    .Dementia as a complication of schizophrenia.J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001;70:588–96.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Bowie CR,
    2. Reichenberg A,
    3. Patterson TL,
    4. et al
    .Determinants of real-world functional performance in schizophrenia subjects: correlations with cognition, functional capacity, and symptoms.Am J Psychiatry 2006;163:418–25.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Harvey PD,
    2. Koren D,
    3. Reichenberg A,
    4. et al
    .Negative symptoms and cognitive deficits: What is the nature of their relationship?.Schizophr Bull 2006;32:250–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Bowie CR,
    2. Depp C,
    3. McGrath JA,
    4. et al
    .Prediction of real-world functional disability in chronic mental disorders: a comparison of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.Am J Psychiatry 2010;167:1116–24.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. MacCabe JH,
    2. Brébion G,
    3. Reichenberg A,
    4. et al
    .Superior intellectual ability in schizophrenia: neuropsychological characteristics.Neuropsychology 2012;26:181–90.
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    1. Koelsch S
    .Toward a neural basis of music perception — a review and updated model.Front Psychol 2011;2:110
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Jeon Y-W,
    2. Polich J
    .Meta-analysis of P300 and schizophrenia: patients, paradigms, and practical implications.Psychophysiology 2003;40:684–701.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Bramon E,
    2. Rabe-Hesketh S,
    3. Sham P,
    4. et al
    .Meta-analysis of the P300 and P50 waveforms in schizophrenia.Schizophr Res 2004;70:315–29.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Umbricht D,
    2. Krljes S
    .Mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis.Schizophr Res 2005;76:1–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Bach DR,
    2. Buxtorf K,
    3. Strik WK,
    4. et al
    .Evidence for impaired sound intensity processing in schizophrenia.Schizophr Bull 2011;37:426–31.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Leitman DI,
    2. Hoptman MJ,
    3. Foxe JJ,
    4. et al
    .The neural substrates of impaired prosodic detection in schizophrenia and its sensorial antecedents.Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:474–82.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Kantrowitz JT,
    2. Leitman DI,
    3. Lehrfeld JM,
    4. et al
    .Reduction in tonal discriminations predicts receptive emotion processing deficits in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder.Schizophr Bull 2013;39:86–93.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Kaneda Y,
    2. Sumiyoshi T,
    3. Keefe R,
    4. et al
    .Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: validation of the Japanese version.Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007;61:602–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    1. Kay SR,
    2. Fiszbein A,
    3. Opler LA
    .The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.Schizophr Bull 1987;13:261–76.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Guy WA
    (1976) ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology (US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Rockville (MD)).
  32. ↵
    1. Inada T
    (1996) Evaluation and diagnosis of Drug-induced Extrapyramidal Symptoms: commentary on the DIEPSS and guide to its usage (Seiwa Shoten, Tokyo).
  33. ↵
    1. Preacher KJ,
    2. Hayes AF
    .Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models.Behav Res Methods 2008;40:879–91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Baron RM,
    2. Kenny DA
    .The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;51:1173–82.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Kraemer HC,
    2. Stice E,
    3. Kazdin A,
    4. et al
    .How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and independent, overlapping, and proxy risk factors.Am J Psychiatry 2001;158:848–56.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Kalmus H,
    2. Fry DB
    .On tune deafness (dysmelodia): frequency, development, genetics and musical background.Ann Hum Genet 1980;43:369–82.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Peretz I,
    2. Cummings S,
    3. Dubé M-P
    .The genetics of congenital amusia (tone deafness): a family-aggregation study.Am J Hum Genet 2007;81:582–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Leitman DI,
    2. Foxe JJ,
    3. Butler PD,
    4. et al
    .Sensory contributions to impaired prosodic processing in schizophrenia.Biol Psychiatry 2005;58:56–61.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Kucharska-Pietura K,
    2. David AS,
    3. Masiak M,
    4. et al
    .Perception of facial and vocal affect by people with schizophrenia in early and late stages of illness.Br J Psychiatry 2005;187:523–8.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  40. ↵
    1. Matsumoto K,
    2. Samson GT,
    3. O’Daly OD,
    4. et al
    .Prosodic discrimination in patients with schizophrenia.Br J Psychiatry 2006;189:180–1.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    1. Peretz I,
    2. Coltheart M
    .Modularity of music processing.Nat Neurosci 2003;6:688–91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Menon V,
    2. Levitin DJ
    .The rewards of music listening: response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system.Neuroimage 2005;28:175–84.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  43. ↵
    1. Salimpoor VN,
    2. Benovoy M,
    3. Larcher K,
    4. et al
    .Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music.Nat Neurosci 2011;14:257–62.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Kapur S,
    2. Mamo D
    .Half a century of antipsychotics and still a central role for dopamine D2 receptors.Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2003;27:1081–90.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Koelsch S
    .Towards a neural basis of music-evoked emotions.Trends Cogn Sci 2010;14:131–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Sawada K,
    2. Barr AM,
    3. Nakamura M,
    4. et al
    .Hippocampal complexin proteins and cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62:263–72.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Honer WG,
    2. Falkai P,
    3. Chen C,
    4. et al
    .Synaptic and plasticity associated proteins in anterior frontal cortex in severe mental illness.Neuroscience 1999;91:1247–55.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Harvey PD,
    2. Lombardi J,
    3. Leibman M,
    4. et al
    .Cognitive impairment and negative symptoms in geriatric chronic schizophrenic patients: a follow-up study.Schizophr Res 1996;22:223–31.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Thoma RJ,
    2. Hanlon FM,
    3. Moses SN,
    4. et al
    .M50 sensory gating predicts negative symptoms in schizophrenia.Schizophr Res 2005;73:311–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. McGrath J,
    2. Chapple B,
    3. Wright M
    .Working memory in schizophrenia and mania: correlation with symptoms during the acute and subacute phases.Acta Psychiatr Scand 2001;103:181–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Mössler K,
    2. Chen X,
    3. Heldal TO,
    4. et al
    .Music therapy for people with schizophrenia and schizophrenia-like disorders.Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2011;12:CD004025
    OpenUrlPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Steinberg R,
    2. Kimmig V,
    3. Raith L,
    4. et al
    .Music psychopathology. IV. The course of musical expression during music therapy with psychiatric inpatients.Psychopathology 1991;24:121–9.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Cramer SC,
    2. Sur M,
    3. Dobkin BH,
    4. et al
    .Harnessing neuroplasticity for clinical application.Brain 2011;134:1591–609.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Koelsch S
    .A neuroscientific perspective on music therapy.Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1169:374–84.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Subramaniam K,
    2. Luks TL,
    3. Fisher M,
    4. et al
    .Computerized cognitive training restores neural activity within the reality monitoring network in schizophrenia.Neuron 2012;73:842–53.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Pajonk F-G,
    2. Wobrock T,
    3. Gruber O,
    4. et al
    .Hippocampal plasticity in response to exercise in schizophrenia.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010;67:133–43.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience: 39 (2)
J Psychiatry Neurosci
Vol. 39, Issue 2
1 Mar 2014
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author

Article tools

Respond to this article
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
To sign up for email alerts or to access your current email alerts, enter your email address below:
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on JPN.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Impaired musical ability in people with schizophrenia
(Your Name) has sent you a message from JPN
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the JPN web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Impaired musical ability in people with schizophrenia
Sanae Hatada, Ken Sawada, Masanori Akamatsu, Erina Doi, Masayoshi Minese, Motoshi Yamashita, Allen E. Thornton, William G. Honer, Shimpei Inoue
J Psychiatry Neurosci Mar 2014, 39 (2) 118-126; DOI: 10.1503/jpn.120207

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
‍ Request Permissions
Share
Impaired musical ability in people with schizophrenia
Sanae Hatada, Ken Sawada, Masanori Akamatsu, Erina Doi, Masayoshi Minese, Motoshi Yamashita, Allen E. Thornton, William G. Honer, Shimpei Inoue
J Psychiatry Neurosci Mar 2014, 39 (2) 118-126; DOI: 10.1503/jpn.120207
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like

Related Articles

  • Correction
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Influence Of Desire To Belong And Feelings Of Loneliness On Emotional Prosody Perception In Schizophrenia
  • Google Scholar

Similar Articles

Content

  • Current issue
  • Past issues
  • Collections
  • Alerts
  • RSS

Authors & Reviewers

  • Overview for Authors
  • Submit a manuscript
  • Manuscript Submission Checklist

About

  • General Information
  • Staff
  • Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertising
  • Reprints
  • Copyright and Permissions
  • Accessibility
  • CMA Civility Standards
CMAJ Group

Copyright 2023, CMA Impact Inc. or its licensors. All rights reserved. ISSN 1180-4882.

All editorial matter in JPN represents the opinions of the authors and not necessarily those of the Canadian Medical Association or its subsidiaries.
To receive any of these resources in an accessible format, please contact us at CMAJ Group, 500-1410 Blair Towers Place, Ottawa ON, K1J 9B9; p: 1-888-855-2555; e: [email protected].
View CMA's Accessibility policy.

Powered by HighWire