Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Issues
    • Issue in progress
    • Issues by date
  • Sections
    • Editorial
    • Review
    • Research
    • Commentary
    • Psychopharmacology for the Clinician
    • Letters to the Editor
  • Topic Collections
  • Instructions for Authors
    • Overview for authors
    • Submission checklist
    • Editorial policies
    • Publication fees
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Dr. Francis Wayne Quan Memorial Prize
    • Open access
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • About
    • General information
    • Staff
    • Editorial Board
    • Contact
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ
    • CMAJ Open
    • CJS
    • JAMC

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
JPN
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ
    • CMAJ Open
    • CJS
    • JAMC
JPN

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Issues
    • Issue in progress
    • Issues by date
  • Sections
    • Editorial
    • Review
    • Research
    • Commentary
    • Psychopharmacology for the Clinician
    • Letters to the Editor
  • Topic Collections
  • Instructions for Authors
    • Overview for authors
    • Submission checklist
    • Editorial policies
    • Publication fees
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Dr. Francis Wayne Quan Memorial Prize
    • Open access
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • About
    • General information
    • Staff
    • Editorial Board
    • Contact
  • Subscribe to our alerts
  • RSS feeds
  • Follow JPN on Twitter
Research Paper

Genetic and environmental influences on corticostriatal circuits in twins with autism

John P. Hegarty, Laura C. Lazzeroni, Mira M. Raman, Joachim F. Hallmayer, Sue C. Cleveland, Olga N. Wolke, Jennifer M. Phillips, Allan L. Reiss and Antonio Y. Hardan
J Psychiatry Neurosci May 01, 2020 45 (3) 188-197; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.190030
John P. Hegarty II
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Hegarty, Lazzeroni, Raman, Hallmayer, Cleveland, Phillips, Reiss, Hardan); the Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Lazzeroni); and the Department of Anesthesiology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Wolke)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Laura C. Lazzeroni
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Hegarty, Lazzeroni, Raman, Hallmayer, Cleveland, Phillips, Reiss, Hardan); the Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Lazzeroni); and the Department of Anesthesiology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Wolke)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mira M. Raman
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Hegarty, Lazzeroni, Raman, Hallmayer, Cleveland, Phillips, Reiss, Hardan); the Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Lazzeroni); and the Department of Anesthesiology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Wolke)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Joachim F. Hallmayer
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Hegarty, Lazzeroni, Raman, Hallmayer, Cleveland, Phillips, Reiss, Hardan); the Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Lazzeroni); and the Department of Anesthesiology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Wolke)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sue C. Cleveland
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Hegarty, Lazzeroni, Raman, Hallmayer, Cleveland, Phillips, Reiss, Hardan); the Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Lazzeroni); and the Department of Anesthesiology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Wolke)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Olga N. Wolke
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Hegarty, Lazzeroni, Raman, Hallmayer, Cleveland, Phillips, Reiss, Hardan); the Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Lazzeroni); and the Department of Anesthesiology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Wolke)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jennifer M. Phillips
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Hegarty, Lazzeroni, Raman, Hallmayer, Cleveland, Phillips, Reiss, Hardan); the Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Lazzeroni); and the Department of Anesthesiology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Wolke)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Allan L. Reiss
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Hegarty, Lazzeroni, Raman, Hallmayer, Cleveland, Phillips, Reiss, Hardan); the Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Lazzeroni); and the Department of Anesthesiology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Wolke)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Antonio Y. Hardan
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Hegarty, Lazzeroni, Raman, Hallmayer, Cleveland, Phillips, Reiss, Hardan); the Department of Biomedical Data Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Lazzeroni); and the Department of Anesthesiology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA (Wolke)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Tables
  • Related Content
  • Responses
  • Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Background: Corticostriatal circuits (CSC) have been implicated in the presentation of some restricted and repetitive behaviours (RRBs) in children with autism-spectrum disorder (ASD), and preliminary evidence suggests that disruptions in these pathways may be associated with differences in genetic and environmental influences on brain development. The objective of this investigation was to examine the impact of genetic and environmental factors on CSC regions in twins with and without ASD and to evaluate their relationship with the severity of RRBs.

Methods: We obtained T1-weighted MRIs from same-sex monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs, aged 6–15 years. Good-quality data were available from 48 ASD pairs (n = 96 twins; 30 pairs concordant for ASD, 15 monozygotic and 15 dizygotic; 18 pairs discordant for ASD, 4 monozygotic and 14 dizygotic) and 34 typically developing control pairs (n = 68 twins; 20 monozygotic and 14 dizygotic pairs). We generated structural measures of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), caudate, putamen, pallidum and thalamus using FreeSurfer. Twin pair comparisons included intraclass correlation analyses and ACE modelling (a2 = additive genetics; c2 = common or shared environment; e2 = unique or nonshared environment). We also assessed correlations with RRB severity.

Results: Structural variation in CSC regions was predominantly genetically mediated in typically developing twins (a2 = 0.56 to 0.87), except for ACC white matter volume (a2 = 0.42, 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.08 to 0.77). We also observed similar magnitudes of genetic influence in twins with ASD (a2 = 0.65 to 0.97), but the cortical thickness of the ACC (c2 = 0.44, 95% CI 0.22 to 0.66) and OFC (c2 = 0.60, 95% CI 0.25 to 0.95) was primarily associated with environmental factors in only twins with ASD. Twin pair differences in OFC grey matter volume were also correlated with RRB severity and were predominantly environmentally mediated.

Limitations: We obtained MRIs on 2 scanners, and analytical approaches could not identify specific genetic and environmental factors.

Conclusion: Genetic factors primarily contribute to structural variation in subcortical CSC regions, regardless of ASD, but environmental factors may exert a greater influence on the development of grey matter thickness in the OFC and ACC in children with ASD. The increased vulnerability of OFC grey matter to environmental influences may also mediate some heterogeneity in RRB severity in children with ASD.

Introduction

Autism-spectrum disorder (ASD) is a prevalent neurodevelopmental disorder; recent estimates indicate that 1 in 40 to 1 in 59 children in the United States currently have a diagnosis.1,2 It is characterized by cognitive and behavioural impairments in multiple symptom domains, primarily social communication impairments and restricted and repetitive patterns of behaviour or interests (RRBs).3 The onset of these symptoms occurs early in life, typically within the first 3 years; however, diagnosable manifestations may not be observable until demands exceed an individual’s limitations. Symptoms of ASD are also extremely variable in presentation and severity across individuals and may originate from impairments across independent domains.4–6 Thus, there are likely multiple subgroups within the ASD population, in which different neurobiological mechanisms may underlie the development of different symptom domains across children with ASD.

Compared with social communication impairments, the RRB symptoms of ASD have received considerably less attention from research investigations. Studies to address this knowledge gap would be beneficial, because RRB symptoms (e.g., repetitive, sensory-motor actions) are present early in development7 and can cause significant management challenges and barriers to adaptive learning later in life.8 Preliminary neurobiological investigations suggest that corticostriatal circuits (CSC), which consist of frontal (orbitofrontal cortex [OFC] and anterior cingulate cortex [ACC]), striatal (caudate, putamen, pallidum) and thalamic regions, are of particular interest for the study of RRB in ASD because these networks are integrally involved with carrying out goal-directed behaviours, and disruption of these pathways is associated with aberrant behavioural presentations.9 For instance, CSC abnormalities are associated with compulsive grooming in mouse models of ASD, and manipulation of this circuit can modulate the severity of this behaviour.10 These findings are also consistent with neuroimaging investigations of CSC regions in people with ASD, which include reports of altered regional growth rates,11 reduced white matter quality,12 decreased functional activation and connectivity,13 and altered neurochemical levels.14 Most importantly, neurobiological alterations in CSC regions (e.g., ACC/OFC,15,16 striatum17,18 and thalamus19,20) have exhibited associations with the severity of RRB symptoms in clinical studies of people with ASD, suggesting that disruptions in CSC processing may mediate some variability in RRB presentation across children on the autism spectrum.

The heterogeneity of RRB symptoms could be associated with the variable effects of genetic and environmental influences on different CSC regions.10,21–24 Preliminary research using the ACE model (a2 = additive genetics; c2 = common or shared environment; e2 = unique or nonshared environment) to quantify the magnitude of genetic and environmental influences on global brain structures suggests that subcortical grey matter may be more genetically mediated in twins with ASD than in typically developing (TD) controls, whereas cortical grey matter, particularly thickness, may be more environmentally mediated.21 These observations suggest that differences in the impact of genetic susceptibility and prenatal/postnatal environmental exposures across CSC regions during development are related to the presentation of RRB in children with ASD. This potential relationship is also supported by animal models of RRB, in which environmental deprivation can increase RRB severity and alter brain development, whereas environmental enrichment can decrease severity and neurobiological alterations.10 To our knowledge, the influence of genetic and environmental factors on the development of CSC regions and RRB severity in children and adolescents with ASD has not yet been evaluated.

The objective of this investigation was to examine ASD-related differences in the influence of genetic25 and environmental26 factors on CSC regions in a sample of twins with and without ASD and evaluate their relationships with RRB severity across multiple behavioural subdomains. Based on previous investigations,27–29 we hypothesized that structural variation in the subcortical grey matter of CSC regions would be primarily genetically mediated in both twins with ASD and TD controls, potentially to a larger extent for twins with ASD, but that structural variation in the frontal grey matter of CSC regions would most likely be associated with environmental factors in only twins with ASD.21 We also expected that differences in RRB severity would be related to differences in the structural properties of CSC regions, such as between repetitive or compulsive behaviours and the grey matter volume of the striatum17,18 and OFC.16 Examining these relationships is important, because it will help account for additional variability in the heterogeneous neurobiological alterations and symptom presentations that are observed across individuals with ASD, which we hope will support advances in early identification and precision medicine.

Methods

Participants

Male and female same-sex twin pairs, in which at least 1 twin was diagnosed with ASD or both were TD, were recruited to participate in this study.21,23,30 Participants with ASD were recruited from the California Autism Twin Study31 and Interactive Autism Network Research Database. Additional ASD and TD twin pairs were also recruited from the local community using online and local advertisements. We confirmed ASD diagnosis with the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R)32 and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd edition (ADOS-2).33 Any twin pairs in which either twin exhibited a genetic or metabolic disorder, an unstable medical condition (such as seizures), a history of traumatic head injury or asphyxia at birth or an MRI contraindication (such as ferrous implants) were excluded from this investigation. In addition, TD twin pairs were excluded if either twin exhibited a severe psychiatric disorder, a history of learning disabilities or a full-scale IQ of less than 70. These criteria were implemented to exclude potential confounding sources of variability in both ASD and TD twin pairs. After screening, 90 twin pairs (i.e., 180 individuals) were invited to participate. We confirmed zygosity from saliva samples based on 9 short tandem repeat loci and the X/Y amelogenin. Monozygotic twin pairs exhibited concordance on all markers, and dizygotic twin pairs were discordant for at least 1 marker.31 Informed consent was obtained from all parents and assent from all children. The methodology of this investigation was approved by the institutional review board.

Psychological testing

We examined general cognitive abilities using the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales, 5th edition,34 and assessed ASD-related impairments using the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS)35 and Restricted Behaviour Scales–Revised (RBS-R),36 which provides subscale measures of multiple domains of repetitive behaviour. We measured handedness using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory37 and quantified socioeconomic status using the Hollingshead method.38

Magnetic resonance imaging

We conducted MRIs on 2 identical GE 3T MR750 scanners at the same institution using a standard 8-channel head coil. Any ASD twins who were unable to lie still for scanning were administered propofol under the supervision of an anesthesiologist at 200–300 μg/kg/min to induce procedural sedation. We acquired 2 T1-weighted inversion recovery spoiled gradient recalled (IR-SPGR) echo pulse sequence images (188 coronal slices, repetition time 8.15 ms, echo time 3.24 ms, inversion time 600 ms, flip angle 12°, slice thickness 1.2 mm, field of view 22 × 22 cm, in-plane resolution 0.86 × 0.86, acquisition matrix size 256 × 192 mm, number of excitations = 1). The highest-quality image was visually identified and then analyzed with FreeSurfer v5.3.39 All automated procedures were inspected by trained raters, who corrected segmentation errors whenever possible; otherwise, the participant was excluded from subsequent analyses. We combined the left and right hemisphere labels from the Desikan–Killiany atlas40 to create regional measures of the OFC (lh_/rh_lateralorbitofrontal + lh_/rh_medialorbitofrontal) and ACC (lh_/rh_rostralanteriorcingulate + lh_/rh_caudalanteriorcingulate) for grey matter and white matter volume, as well as surface area. We also evaluated average grey matter thickness across labels for the OFC and ACC, and grey matter volumes of the left and right caudate, putamen, pallidum and thalamus labels were combined.

Two sets of twins were assessed on both scanners, and repeated-measures comparisons indicated an approximately 6% difference in total brain volume across sites Prior to Free-Surfer segmentation, affected scans (ASD and TD) were transformed using the FSL linear transformation package FLIRT41 with standard sinc interpolation to FSL standard orientation images using a matrix that was designed to minimize potential scanner effects across all global structural measures that were evaluated.21 Importantly, both twins from each pair were always scanned at the same location, and statistical analyses were based primarily on comparisons within twin pairs.

Statistical analysis

We generated intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) of structural data, adjusted for sex and diagnosis, using STATA42 and the DeFries–Fulker model43 for all monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs to evaluate the general assumptions of twin modelling. Next, we examined the ICCs separately for the ASD and TD subgroups, adjusting for sex and excluding twin pairs discordant for ASD, and we compared them with Fisher z-transformation44 to provide an initial assessment for the magnitude of differences between diagnostic groups. Twin pairs discordant for ASD were excluded from these analyses because they may differ in environmental factors to a greater degree than concordant twin pairs, and our goal was to provide a more representative estimation of potential ASD-related differences in genetic and environmental influences on structural properties of the CSC regions. We then calculated the ACE model for broad sense heritability based on Falconer’s formula45 to estimate the contribution of genetic and environmental influences to structural variation in CSC regions. The ACE model estimates the proportion of variation in a trait of interest (e.g., OFC grey matter thickness or caudate grey matter volume) that is related to different factors (a2 = additive genetics; c2 = common or shared environment; e2 = unique or nonshared environment) by comparing trait variability in monozygotic versus dizygotic twin pairs who share many environmental features and differ in genetic influences by a known quantity (i.e., approximately 50%). We also assessed Pearson correlations of within-twin-pair differences in regional measures and symptom severity, as assessed with the SRS and RBS-R, to evaluate genetic and environmental influences on these brain–behaviour relationships. We used false discovery rate46 correction to account for multiple comparisons.

Results

Participants

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the twin samples can be found in Table 1.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Demographics and clinical characteristics*

Following confirmation of quality-assurance criteria, anatomic scans from 30 twin pairs in which both twins had ASD (15 monozygotic, 15 dizygotic), 18 twin pairs discordant for ASD (4 monozygotic, 14 dizygotic) and 34 TD twin pairs (20 monozygotic, 14 dizygotic) were available for analysis (n = 164 twins). Discordance for ASD was defined as 1 twin meeting diagnostic criteria on the ADI-R32 and ADOS-2,33 and the other twin not meeting criteria for either ASD or exhibiting subthreshold ASD-related impairments. Comparing concordant ASD (n = 30 pairs) and TD (n = 34 pairs) twin pairs, SES was slightly lower in ASD twin pairs than in TD controls (mean difference between groups [M] = −5.29, 95% CI −10.51 to −0.72), predominantly owing to dizygotic twin pairs (M = −10.06, 95% CI −18.11 to −2.01). We found no other zygosity × diagnostic group differences, and adjusting for SES did not significantly alter the twin modelling results, indicating that the groups were relatively well matched. Although we observed some within-diagnostic-group differences in SRS scores between dizygotic (ASD n = 58; TD n = 28) and monozygotic (ASD n = 38; TD n = 40) twins (ASD M = −7.39, 95% CI −14.26 to −0.13; TD M = −4.28, 95% CI −6.82 to −1.74), TD twins were below the threshold for any ASD-related impairments,35 and ASD subgroup differences were likely related to the larger number of dizygotic twin pairs discordant for ASD.

Comparison of CSC structural measures across diagnostic groups

Diagnostic group comparisons of regional structural data are included in the supplementary materials (Appendix 1, Table S1 and Table S2, available at jpn.ca/190030-a1). As expected, we observed only moderate ASD-related differences, which indicated larger grey matter volume of the striatal regions (putamen M = 431.81, 95% CI 9.95 to 853.67) but thinner cortical grey matter (OFC M = −0.07, 95% CI −0.13 to −0.004) in twins with ASD (n = 78) compared with TD controls (n = 68). Interestingly, thalamic grey matter volume was smaller (M = 1125.86, 95% CI 249.44 to 2002.28) and OFC grey matter was thicker (M = 0.12, 95% CI 0.03 to 0.20) in twins with ASD compared with unaffected co-twins (n = 18 pairs), suggesting that greater control over potentially confounding sources of variability (e.g., shared environmental influences) can provide additional insights into ASD-related alterations.

ICCs of CSC structural measures in monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs

In all twin pairs, regardless of diagnosis, ICCs were positive, statistically significant and higher in monozygotic twin pairs (n = 78 twins/39 pairs) than in dizygotic twin pairs (n = 86 twins/43 pairs; p < 0.05 in most instances; Appendix 1, Table S3). The only exceptions were zygosity subgroup comparisons for the ACC, in which white matter volume (ICC monozygotic = 0.93, ICC dizygotic = 0.95, z = −1.09, p = 0.28), grey matter surface area (ICC monozygotic = 0.95, ICC dizygotic = 0.92, z = 1.52, p = 0.13) and cortical thickness (ICC monozygotic = 0.65, ICC dizygotic = 0.53, z = 1.16, p = 0.25) were not statistically different in monozygotic compared with dizygotic twins (see Discussion).

The ICC estimates for monozygotic ASD twin pairs (n = 30 twins/15 pairs) and TD twin pairs (n = 40 twins/20 pairs; Fig. 1; Appendix 1, Table S4) were also generally of large magnitude (approximately 0.50 to 0.90) and statistically significant (p < 0.01) in all instances, with the exception of ACC grey matter thickness in ASD twin pairs (ICC = 0.35, 95% CI −0.03 to 0.73) and white matter volume in TD twin pairs (ICC = 0.49, 95% CI 0.013 to 0.84). Comparing monozygotic twins across groups, ICCs were larger in monozygotic ASD twin pairs compared with monozygotic TD twin pairs for ACC white matter volume (ICC ASD = 0.91, ICC TD = 0.49, z = 3.92, p < 0.001) and grey matter surface area (ICC ASD = 0.91, ICC TD = 0.60, z = 3.30, p = 0.001), as well as grey matter volume of the caudate (ICC ASD = 0.91, ICC TD = 0.69, z = 2.68, p = 0.01) and putamen (ICC ASD = 0.90, ICC TD = 0.71, z = 2.31, p = 0.02). Conversely, the ICC for ACC grey matter thickness was larger in monozygotic TD twins compared with monozygotic ASD twins (ICC ASD = 0.35, ICC TD = 0.81, z = −3.01, p = 0.003). These differences were generally of large magnitude and survived correction for multiple comparisons.

Fig. 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 1

Intraclass correlations in ACE model space (A = additive genetics; C = common or shared environment; E = unique or nonshared environment; D = genetic dominance). Intraclass correlation coefficients within twin pairs, in which both twins were diagnosed with autism-spectrum disorder (ASD0 or were typically developing (TD) controls (adjusted for sex), were generated within monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs (ASD: monozygotic n = 15 pairs, dizygotic n = 15 pairs; TD monozygotic n = 20 pairs, dizygotic n = 14 pairs) and displayed in relation to ACE model space. Brain structures near or above the CE line (shaded) are primarily environmentally mediated, whereas brain structures near or below the DE line (shaded) are primarily genetically mediated. This figure provides a visualization of the data used to generate ACE model parameter estimates. ACC = anterior cingulate cortex; ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient; OFC = orbitofrontal cortex.

The ICC estimates were generally of lower magnitude (approximately −0.25 to 0.60) for dizygotic ASD twin pairs (n = 30 twins/15 pairs) and TD twin pairs (n = 28 twins/14 pairs; Fig. 1; Appendix 1, Table S4) compared with monozygotic twin pairs, and only a subset was statistically significant and lower in the dizygotic compared with the monozygotic subgroups at p < 0.05. Dizygotic ASD twins exhibited higher ICCs than dizygotic TD twins for ACC white matter volume (ICC ASD = 0.59, ICC TD = 0.09, z = 2.12, p = 0.03) and ACC (ICC ASD = 0.59, ICC TD = −0.02, z = 2.51, p = 0.01) and OFC grey matter surface area (ICC ASD = 0.39, ICC TD = −0.16, z = 2.07, p = 0.04). The diagnostic group comparisons between dizygotic twin pairs did not survive correction for multiple comparisons, but differences across the monozygotic and dizygotic subgroups for the ACC may have contributed to the overall twin pair observations outlined above.

ACE modelling of CSC structural measures

The ACE model provides estimates for the proportion of variation in a trait of interest that is related to additive genetic factors (a2), common/shared environmental factors (c2) or unique (e2) environmental factors (Table 2). Under a primarily genetic AE model, the ICCs in monozygotic twins are approximately twice that of dizygotic twins, whereas under a primarily environmental CE model, ICCs in monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs are nearly equal. Within TD twin pairs, all structural measures of CSC regions that could be modelled were best fit with an AE model and exhibited significant genetic contributions (a2 = 0.56 to 0.87). Anterior cingulate cortex and OFC grey matter surface area, as well as the OFC white matter volume, could not be fit due to negative ICCs in the dizygotic TD subgroup; however, variation in these measures also appeared to be primarily genetically mediated based on ICC comparisons. Anterior cingulate cortex white matter volume was also best fit with the AE model in TD twins, but the majority of variation was attributed to the environmental factor (a2 = 0.42). No structural measures from CSC regions were best fit with the CE model in TD twins.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

ACE model parameter estimates for concordant ASD and TD twin pairs*

In ASD twin pairs, we found some significant deviations from the primarily genetically mediated contributions that were identified in TD controls. Grey matter and white matter volume, as well as the surface area of all CSC regions, were also best fit with the AE model, and primarily genetically mediated in ASD twin pairs (a2 = 0.65 to 0.97); however, ACC and OFC grey matter thickness were best fit with the CE model (c2 = 0.44 to 0.60). This observation suggests that environmental factors may predominantly contribute to cortical thickness of the ACC and OFC in twins with ASD. Thalamic grey matter volume could not be modelled in ASD twin pairs because of a negative ICC in the dizygotic subgroup, but ICC comparisons suggested that it was also primarily genetically mediated, consistent with a previous study that examined markers of neuronal integrity in this structure in twins with ASD.23

RRB symptomatology and its relationship with CSC regions

Twins from ASD pairs exhibited more severe RRB symptoms than TD control twins (SRS M = 22.56, 95% CI 18.50 to 26.63; RBS-R M = 17.86, 95% CI 13.25 to 22.47) across all RRB subdomains that were assessed, including the stereotyped (M = 3.13, 95% CI 2.33 to 3.92), self-injurious (M = 2.20, 95% CI 1.40 to 3.00), compulsive (M = 2.94, 95% CI 1.89 to 4.00), ritualistic (M = 3.19, 95% CI 2.23 to 4.14), sameness (M = 3.87, 95% CI 2.46 to 5.29) and restricted behaviours (M = 2.35, 95% CI 1.58 to 3.11) subscales of the RBS-R. Interestingly, we also found relevant differences in RRB severity in monozygotic twin pairs concordant for ASD (SRS M = 12.73, 95% CI 5.79 to 19.67; RBS-R M = 8.60, 95% CI 4.47 to 12.73), including stereotyped (M = 1.40, 95% CI 0.45 to 2.36), sameness (M = 1.47, 95% CI 0.29 to 2.65) and restricted behaviours (M = 1.67, 95% CI 0.59 to 2.75), but only moderate differences in compulsive (M = 1.80, 95% CI −0.18 to 3.78) and ritualistic behaviours (M = 1.00, 95% CI −0.25 to 2.03). These observations suggest that nongenetic factors may contribute to RRB severity in children and adolescents with ASD. The influence of nongenetic factors on RRB severity was further supported by ACE modelling in children and adolescents with ASD for sameness (c2 = 0.89, 95% CI 0.74 to 1.04, p < 0.001) and restricted behaviours (c2 = 0.53, 95% CI 0.15 to 0.91, p < 0.006), but stereotyped behaviours appeared to also have significant genetic contributions (a2 = 0.74, 95% CI 0.47 to 1.01, p < 0.001).

Correlations of twin pair differences in structural measures and symptom severity also indicated some interesting brain–behaviour relationships between CSC and RRB (Fig. 2). The primary associations identified were between differences in OFC grey matter volume and RRB severity in twins with ASD. Across all dizygotic twin pairs, regardless of diagnostic group, OFC grey matter volume was associated with the severity of autistic mannerisms (r = 0.43, 95% CI 0.14 to 0.65, p = 0.005), stereotyped behaviours (r = 0.44, 95% CI 0.15 to 0.66, p = 0.005) and compulsive behaviours (r = 0.38, 95% CI 0.08 to 0.61, p = 0.016). These relationships appeared to be primarily owing to dizygotic ASD twins (autistic mannerisms r = 0.51, 95% CI 0.17 to 0.74, p = 0.005; stereotyped behaviours r = 0.55, 95% CI 0.21 to 0.77, p = 0.003; compulsive behaviours r= 0.41, 95% CI 0.05 to 0.68, p = 0.029) and not TD controls (p > 0.05 in all instances for this group). We also observed similar relationships for monozygotic twin pairs, regardless of diagnostic group, for OFC grey matter volume and the severity of autistic mannerisms (r = 0.40, 95% CI 0.09 to 0.64, p = 0.013) and stereotyped behaviours (r = 0.43, 95% CI 0.13 to 0.66, p = 0.007), but not compulsive behaviours (r = 0.30, 95% CI −0.02 to 0.57, p = 0.07). These relationships also appeared to be primarily owing to monozygotic ASD twins (autistic mannerisms r = 0.54, 95% CI 0.10 to 0.80, p = 0.021; stereotyped behaviours r = 0.62, 95% CI 0.21 to 0.84, p = 0.006) and not TD controls (p > 0.05 in all instances). The similarity between monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs with ASD suggests that environmental factors may also contribute to the relationship between CSC alterations and RRB severity in children and adolescents with ASD.

Fig. 2
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Fig. 2

Relationships between twin pair differences in grey matter volume and severity of restricted and repetitive behaviours (RRB). Within-twin-pair differences in grey matter volume (mm3) of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) are displayed in relation to RRB, as assessed by the autistic mannerisms subscale of the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) and the stereotyped behaviours subscale of the Restricted Behaviour Subscales – Revised (RBS-R), for dizygotic and monozygotic twin pairs in which at least 1 twin was diagnosed with autism-spectrum disorder (ASD) or both were typically developing (TD) controls (ASD: monozygotic n = 19 pairs, dizygotic n = 29 pairs; TD: monozygotic n = 20 pairs, dizygotic n = 14 pairs). Pearson correlations that were significant at p < 0.05 (solid line) are displayed for twin pairs with ASD. These correlations were also significant across all individuals but not separately in TD control twins.

Discussion

Complex interactions between genetic and environmental factors during development may mediate some of the variability in neurobiological alterations and symptom presentation across individuals with ASD,21,25,26 such as the relationship between CSC and RRB.9,10 In the current study, we found substantial genetic influences on subcortical (i.e., striatum and thalamus) and frontal (i.e., ACC and OFC) CSC regions in both twins with ASD and TD controls, but cortical thickness of grey matter in the ACC and OFC was predominantly influenced by environmental factors in only twins with ASD. Differences in cortical grey matter of the OFC were also associated with the severity of RRB symptoms, as assessed by 2 independent measures, and comparisons across zygosity subgroups indicated that environmental factors most likely contributed to these brain–behaviour relationships. Potential environmental contributions to the relationship between CSC and RRB were further supported by the observation of differences in RRB severity in monozygotic twin pairs with ASD, who share a similar genetic background, as well as differences in grey matter thickness of the OFC in twin pairs discordant for ASD, who provide greater control for shared environmental influences. Cumulatively, our findings suggest that genetic and environmental factors differentially affect CSC structure in children with ASD, with genetic factors primarily contributing to subcortical development but environmental factors contributing to the development of frontal lobe grey matter to a larger extent in children with ASD compared with TD controls.

Previously, CSC have been implicated in ASD with reports of neurobiological alterations in this network compared with TD controls.11–20 Although reports vary across studies and developmental periods,47 CSC differences are relatively well supported across both basic science and clinical studies of ASD.9 Observations from the current study further support that there are neurobiological differences in CSC structures in children and adolescents with ASD and included indications of larger grey matter volume of the putamen, smaller grey matter volume of the thalamus and reduced cortical thickness of the OFC in twins with ASD compared with TD controls. However, these differences were only moderate and did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. Grey matter volume of the thalamus was also moderately reduced in twins with ASD compared with unaffected (discordant) co-twins, whereas cortical thickness of the OFC was moderately increased. Because co-twins provide greater control for potential confounding sources of variability, these findings suggest that differences in genetic and environmental contributions to the development of CSC regions may mediate some heterogeneity in ASD-related alterations across individuals. Thus, examining the influence of genetic and environmental factors on CSC regions in twins with ASD is an important step toward improving neurobiological models of this disorder.

The effects of genetic and environmental influences on brain development typically exhibit dynamic changes during different developmental periods. For instance, environmental contributions generally increase with age in evolutionary older structures, such as regions associated with basic sensory processing, but decrease with age in more recently evolved regions, such as prefrontal regions associated with higher-order processes such as executive control and language.24 Preliminary studies of twins with ASD suggest that this shift in genetic versus environmental control of brain development may be altered in children with ASD.21 Observations from the current study support these previous reports regarding genetic and environmental influences on global brain structure in twins with ASD and further indicate that subcortical CSC grey matter is primarily genetically mediated in children and adolescents with and without ASD, potentially to an even larger extent in individuals with ASD. Conversely, cortical CSC grey matter, particularly cortical thickness, may be primarily environmentally mediated in children and adolescents with ASD but not TD controls. Interestingly, investigations of young infants at high risk for ASD report that alterations in grey matter surface area are present very early during postnatal life, whereas alterations in cortical thickness may occur later during postnatal development.48 Thus, our findings suggest that the postnatal environment may have a larger impact on the development of the ACC and OFC in children with ASD, which could be a contributing factor to symptom presentation.

Neurobiological alterations in CSC regions, including the prefrontal cortex, are associated with RRB severity,15–20 suggesting that disruptions in CSC processing may mediate some of the extreme heterogeneity in symptom presentation across individuals with ASD. In the current investigation, we also found that twin pair differences in grey matter volume of the OFC were correlated with differences in the severity of autistic mannerisms, as measured by the SRS, as well as the stereotyped and compulsive behaviours, as measured by the RBS-R. These observations support the concept that structural variation in CSC regions, particularly the OFC,16 contribute to RRB severity in children and adolescents with ASD. Furthermore, these brain–behaviour relationships were very similar in monozygotic twin pairs, who share 100% of their genetic profiles, and dizygotic twin pairs, who differ by approximately 50%, which suggests that environmental factors may affect the relationship between grey matter development and RRB severity. The existence of differences in RRB severity in monozygotic twins concordant for ASD, as well as ACE modelling of RRB subdomains, further supports a relationship between environmental factors and RRB presentation. These clinical observations are also supported by animal models of RRB in which environmental deprivation can increase RRB expression, whereas environmental enrichment can reverse the phenotype.10 Overall, genetic factors may generally increase vulnerability for the development of ASD and contribute to the severity of stereotyped behaviours, potentially via pathways that affect cortical white matter, grey matter surface area21,48 and subcortical CSC regions, whereas environmental influences could play a role in the expression and severity of other RRB symptom domains through mechanisms that affect cerebellar white matter and cortical thickness.21,22 Additional research will be necessary to elucidate these complex brain–behaviour-environment interactions, but preliminary evidence from the current investigation suggests that the postnatal environment contributes to the relationship between CSC alterations and RRB presentation in children with ASD, which warrants further investigation.

Limitations

Several important limitations should be considered regarding the interpretation of our findings. First, our analyses applied a rather basic twin modelling approach because our sample was not large enough to apply more advanced twin modelling techniques, evident from the observation of nonsignificant or negative ICCs for several structural measures. Second, the examination of related individuals could have affected our general diagnostic group comparisons, but the evaluation of co-twins discordant for ASD also provided an especially powerful control group for potentially confounding sources of variability. Third, the use of 2 separate scanners could have introduced additional variability into our comparisons, although transformation of approximately one-third of the neuroimaging data was applied, and analyses based on a single scanner did not significantly differ from those for the full data set.21 Finally, our findings indicate the level of magnitude with which genetic and environmental factors may contribute to the development of CSC structure, as well as the relationship between CSC alterations and RRB severity, but not the specific mechanisms that contribute to these brain–behaviour relationships (e.g., genetic polymorphisms, DNA methylation levels, in utero drug exposure, perinatal complications or postnatal environmental exposures).

Conclusion

Brain structure is primarily genetically medicated during typical development, but there are also periods of increased sensitivity to environmental factors during different developmental periods. This is illustrated by the increased influence of environmental factors on lower-order sensory processing regions during postnatal development and diminished influence of environmental factors on higher-order frontal regions throughout the lifespan.24 Our findings indicate that these dynamic genetic and environmental interactions may be altered in children and adolescents with ASD, such that genetic contributions could have a greater impact on the development of subcortical grey matter, whereas environmental contributions could have a greater impact on the development of the frontal lobe, which may be susceptible to environmental influences for a longer period of time in children with ASD compared with TD controls. Environmental influences on the OFC may also mediate some heterogeneity in the presentation of RRB across individuals. Future investigations should assess the specific mechanisms that contribute to these complex interactions and identify the developmental periods and brain structures that may be particularly vulnerable to their effects. This will be particularly important for improving neurobiological modelling of ASD to increase the potential for neurobiological stratification in the future.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participants and their families, many of whom travelled great distances, for their involvement in this research.

Footnotes

  • Funding: This work was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH083972 to A.Y.H.), and J. Hegarty was supported by the Bass Society Pediatric Fellowship Program. These funding sources had no involvement in this research, and the authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

  • Competing interests: None declared.

  • Contributors: L. Lazzeroni, J. Hallmayer, A. Reiss and A. Hardan designed the study. J. Hegarty, M. Raman, S. Cleveland, O. Wolke, J. Phillips, A. Reiss and A. Hardan acquired the data, which J. Hegarty, L. Lazzeroni, M. Raman and A. Hardan analyzed. J. Hegarty and A. Hardan wrote the article, which all authors reviewed. All authors approved the final version to be published and can certify that no other individuals not listed as authors have made substantial contributions to the paper.

  • Received February 12, 2019.
  • Revision received April 2, 2019.
  • Accepted June 10, 2019.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Baio J,
    2. Wiggins L,
    3. Christensen DL,
    4. et al
    .Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United States, 2014.MMWR Surveill Summ 2018;67:1–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Kogan MD,
    2. Vladutiu CJ,
    3. Schieve LA,
    4. et al
    .The prevalence of parent-reported autism spectrum disorder among US children.Pediatrics 2018;142:e20174161
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    1. American Psychiatric Association
    (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (APA, Arlington (VA)), 5th ed (DSM-5).
  4. ↵
    1. Mandy WP,
    2. Skuse DH
    .Research review: what is the association between the social-communication element of autism and repetitive interests, behaviours and activities?.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008;49:795–808.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Ronald A,
    2. Happ ÉF,
    3. Bolton P,
    4. et al
    .Genetic heterogeneity between the three components of the autism spectrum: a twin study.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2006;45:691–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Ronald A,
    2. Happé F,
    3. Plomin R
    .The genetic relationship between individual differences in social and nonsocial behaviours characteristic of autism.Dev Sci 2005;8:444–58.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Baranek GT
    .Autism during infancy: a retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and social behaviors at 9–12 months of age.J Autism Dev Disord 1999;29:213–24.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Leekam SR,
    2. Prior MR,
    3. Uljarevic M
    .Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: a review of research in the last decade.Psychol Bull 2011;137:562–93.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    1. Lewis M,
    2. Kim S-J
    .The pathophysiology of restricted repetitive behavior.J Neurodev Disord 2009;1:114–32.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    1. Lewis MH,
    2. Tanimura Y,
    3. Lee LW,
    4. et al
    .Animal models of restricted repetitive behavior in autism.Behav Brain Res 2007;176:66–74.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Langen M,
    2. Bos D,
    3. Noordermeer SD,
    4. et al
    .Changes in the development of striatum are involved in repetitive behavior in autism.Biol Psychiatry 2014;76:405–11.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Shukla DK,
    2. Keehn B,
    3. Müller R-A
    .Tract-specific analyses of diffusion tensor imaging show widespread white matter compromise in autism spectrum disorder.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011;52:286–95.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Delmonte S,
    2. Gallagher L,
    3. O’Hanlon E,
    4. et al
    .Functional and structural connectivity of frontostriatal circuitry in autism spectrum disorder.Front Hum Neurosci 2013;7:430
    OpenUrl
  13. ↵
    1. Horder J,
    2. Lavender T,
    3. Mendez MA,
    4. et al
    .Reduced subcortical glutamate/ glutamine in adults with autism spectrum disorders: a [1H]MRS study.Transl Psychiatry 2013;3:e279
    OpenUrl
  14. ↵
    1. Thakkar KN,
    2. Polli FE,
    3. Joseph RM,
    4. et al
    .Response monitoring, repetitive behaviour and anterior cingulate abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASD).Brain 2008;131:2464–78.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Hardan AY,
    2. Girgis RR,
    3. Lacerda ALT,
    4. et al
    .Magnetic resonance imaging study of the orbitofrontal cortex in autism.J Child Neurol 2006;21:866–71.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Hollander E,
    2. Anagnostou E,
    3. Chaplin W,
    4. et al
    .Striatal volume on magnetic resonance imaging and repetitive behaviors in autism.Biol Psychiatry 2005;58:226–32.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Sears LL,
    2. Vest C,
    3. Mohamed S,
    4. et al
    .An MRI study of the basal ganglia in autism.Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1999;23:613–24.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Duerden EG,
    2. Card D,
    3. Roberts SW,
    4. et al
    .Self-injurious behaviours are associated with alterations in the somatosensory system in children with autism spectrum disorder.Brain Struct Funct 2014;219:1251–61.
    OpenUrl
  19. ↵
    1. Estes A,
    2. Shaw DWW,
    3. Sparks BF,
    4. et al
    .Basal ganglia morphometry and repetitive behavior in young children with autism spectrum disorder.Autism Res 2011;4:212–20.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Hegarty JP II.,
    2. Pegoraro LFL,
    3. Lazzeroni LC,
    4. et al
    .Genetic and environmental influences on structural brain measures in twins with autism spectrum disorder.Mol Psychiatry 2019January18[Epub ahead of print]doi:10.1038/s41380-018-0330-z
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  21. ↵
    1. Kates WR,
    2. Burnette CP,
    3. Eliez S,
    4. et al
    .Neuroanatomic variation in monozygotic twin pairs discordant for the narrow phenotype for autism.Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:539–46.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Hegarty JP,
    2. Gu M,
    3. Spielman DM,
    4. et al
    .A proton MR spectroscopy study of the thalamus in twins with autism spectrum disorder.Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2018;81:153–60.
    OpenUrl
  23. ↵
    1. Lenroot RK,
    2. Schmitt JE,
    3. Ordaz SJ,
    4. et al
    .Differences in genetic and environmental influences on the human cerebral cortex associated with development during childhood and adolescence.Hum Brain Mapp 2007;30:163–74.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  24. ↵
    1. Miles JH
    .Autism spectrum disorders — a genetics review.Genet Med 2011;13:278
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Modabbernia A,
    2. Velthorst E,
    3. Reichenberg A
    .Environmental risk factors for autism: an evidence-based review of systematic reviews and meta-analyses.Mol Autism 2017;8:13
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Pennington BF,
    2. Filipek PA,
    3. Lefly D,
    4. et al
    .A Twin MRI study of size variations in the human brain.J Cogn Neurosci 2000;12:223–32.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Yoon U,
    2. Fahim C,
    3. Perusse D,
    4. et al
    .Lateralized genetic and environmental influences on human brain morphology of 8-year-old twins.Neuroimage 2010;53:1117–25.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Blokland GAM,
    2. de Zubicaray GI,
    3. McMahon KL,
    4. et al
    .Genetic and environmental influences on neuroimaging phenotypes: a meta-analytical perspective on twin imaging studies.Twin Res Hum Genet 2012;15:351–71.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Monterrey JC,
    2. Philips J,
    3. Cleveland S,
    4. et al
    .Incidental brain MRI findings in an autism twin study.Autism Res 2017;10:113–20.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Hallmayer J,
    2. Cleveland S,
    3. Torres A,
    4. et al
    .Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism.JAMA Psychiatry 2011;68:1095–102.
    OpenUrl
  30. ↵
    1. Lord C,
    2. Rutter M,
    3. Couteur A
    .Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders.J Autism Dev Disord 1994;24:659–85.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Lord C,
    2. Rutter M,
    3. DiLavore P,
    4. et al.
    (2012) Autism diagnostic observation schedule (Western Psychological Corporation, Torrance (CA)), 2nd ed (ADOS-2).
  32. ↵
    1. Roid GH
    (2003) Stanford–Binet intelligence scales (Riverside Publishing, Itasca (IL)).
  33. ↵
    1. Constantino JN,
    2. Gruber CP
    (2007) Social responsiveness scale (SRS) (Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles).
  34. ↵
    1. Lam KS,
    2. Aman MG
    .The Repetitive Behavior Scale – Revised: independent validation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders.J Autism Dev Disord 2007;37:855–66.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Oldfield RC
    .The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.Neuropsychologia 1971;9:97–113.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Hollingshead AB
    (1975) Four factor index of social status (Yale University, New Haven (CT)).
  37. ↵
    1. Fischl B
    .FreeSurfer.Neuroimage 2012;62:774–81.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    1. Desikan RS,
    2. Ségonne F,
    3. Fischl B,
    4. et al
    .An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest.Neuroimage 2006;31:968–80.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Jenkinson M,
    2. Bannister P,
    3. Brady M,
    4. et al
    .Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images.Neuroimage 2002;17:825–41.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    (2017) Stata statistical software (StataCorp LLC, College Station (TX)) Release 15.
  41. ↵
    1. DeFries JC,
    2. Fulker DW
    .Multiple regression analysis of twin data.Behav Genet 1985;15:467–73.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    1. Fisher RA
    .Frequency distribution of the values of the correlation coefficient in samples from an indefinitely large population.Biometrika 1915;10:507–21.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  43. ↵
    1. Falconer DS
    (1981) Introduction to quantitative genetics (Longman, London), 2nd ed.
  44. ↵
    1. Benjamini Y,
    2. Hochberg Y
    .Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing.J R Stat Soc Ser A Stat Soc 1995;57:289–300.
    OpenUrl
  45. ↵
    1. Brambilla P,
    2. Hardan A,
    3. di Nemi SU,
    4. et al
    .Brain anatomy and development in autism: review of structural MRI studies.Brain Res Bull 2003;61:557–69.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Hazlett HC,
    2. Gu H,
    3. Munsell BC,
    4. et al
    .Early brain development in infants at high risk for autism spectrum disorder.Nature 2017;542:348
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience: 45 (3)
J Psychiatry Neurosci
Vol. 45, Issue 3
1 May 2020
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author

Article tools

Respond to this article
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
To sign up for email alerts or to access your current email alerts, enter your email address below:
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on JPN.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Genetic and environmental influences on corticostriatal circuits in twins with autism
(Your Name) has sent you a message from JPN
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the JPN web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Genetic and environmental influences on corticostriatal circuits in twins with autism
John P. Hegarty, Laura C. Lazzeroni, Mira M. Raman, Joachim F. Hallmayer, Sue C. Cleveland, Olga N. Wolke, Jennifer M. Phillips, Allan L. Reiss, Antonio Y. Hardan
J Psychiatry Neurosci May 2020, 45 (3) 188-197; DOI: 10.1503/jpn.190030

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
‍ Request Permissions
Share
Genetic and environmental influences on corticostriatal circuits in twins with autism
John P. Hegarty, Laura C. Lazzeroni, Mira M. Raman, Joachim F. Hallmayer, Sue C. Cleveland, Olga N. Wolke, Jennifer M. Phillips, Allan L. Reiss, Antonio Y. Hardan
J Psychiatry Neurosci May 2020, 45 (3) 188-197; DOI: 10.1503/jpn.190030
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like

Related Articles

  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • No citing articles found.
  • Google Scholar

Similar Articles

Content

  • Current issue
  • Past issues
  • Collections
  • Alerts
  • RSS

Authors & Reviewers

  • Overview for Authors
  • Submit a manuscript
  • Manuscript Submission Checklist

About

  • General Information
  • Staff
  • Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertising
  • Reprints
  • Copyright and Permissions
  • Accessibility
  • CMA Civility Standards
CMAJ Group

Copyright 2023, CMA Impact Inc. or its licensors. All rights reserved. ISSN 1180-4882.

All editorial matter in JPN represents the opinions of the authors and not necessarily those of the Canadian Medical Association or its subsidiaries.
To receive any of these resources in an accessible format, please contact us at CMAJ Group, 500-1410 Blair Towers Place, Ottawa ON, K1J 9B9; p: 1-888-855-2555; e: [email protected].
View CMA's Accessibility policy.

Powered by HighWire