Research report
Relationship between perception of facial emotions and anxiety in clinical depression: does anxiety-related perception predict persistence of depression?

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(97)01432-8Get rights and content

Abstract

Within the framework of interpersonal theories on depression, it was postulated 1) that an anxiety-related mood-congruent bias with respect to the perception of facial expressions could be demonstrated in clinically depressed patients; 2) that the perception of negative facial emotions would be associated with co-occurring anxiety levels rather than with depression, and 3) that the putative anxiety-related bias would predict the subsequent course of depression. Such relationships would support the possible causal role of negative biases for the persistence of depression.

Thirty-nine depressed patients (thirty-six patients met the criteria for major depression, two had a dysthymic disorder and one patient suffered from a cyclothymic disorder) were studied. The patients judged schematic faces with respect to the emotions they express (fear, happiness, anger, sadness, disgust, surprise, rejection and invitation) at admission (T0), and after 6 (T1) and 30 (T2) weeks. Severity of depression (BDI) and anxiety (SCL-90) were assessed at these three points.

We found considerable support for the first 2 hypotheses: a) The perception of negative emotions was related to anxiety but not to depression (at T0 this association was significant and at T1 and T2 tendencies were found); b) When the level of depression was controlled for, significant relationships remained (emerged) between anxiety and the perception of negative emotions at each of the three different time points; c) Anxiety and perception of negative emotions covaried within subjects when large changes in depression/anxiety were involved, i.e. after 30 weeks. This relationship disappeared when depression change was partialled out. The third hypothesis was not confirmed: The perception of negative emotions did not predict the course of depression.

Although a direct relationship with depression persistence and a negative bias in the perception of interaction-relevant stimuli (i.e. facial emotions) in anxious depressed patients could not be found, the existence of such anxiety-related negative bias forms indirect evidence for the notion that this negative bias may mediate rejective attitudes of others towards depressives and consequently may contribute to an unfavourable course of depression.

Introduction

Interpersonal processes are presumed to play a role in depression persistence. Deficient or problematic social behaviours of depression-prone persons may underlie depressogenic processes by eliciting negative reactions in others, which may result in withdrawal by family and friends (Lewinsohn, 1974, Coyne et al., 1990, Segrin and Abramson, 1994). A solid body of evidence supports rejection attitudes of others towards depressed subjects (Segrin, 1993a, Segrin and Abramson, 1994). However, which processes underlie the emergence of such negative reactions in others is unknown. According to Beck's model, depression is characterized by mood-congruent biases, which operate all aspects of processing, such as attention, reasoning, memory (Beck et al., 1979). The cognitive system “filters” environmental information through imposing pre-existing memory representations on this new information. It has frequently been suggested that such enhanced processing of negative cues plays a role in the persistence of depression (Gotlib and Hammen, 1992, Mathews and McLeod, 1994). One may argue that the depression-prone person's perception of how (significant) others communicate emotions may be relevant for depression onset and persistence: a negative bias in the perception of interaction-relevant stimuli may serve as a mediator of rejecting attitudes of significant others. Knowing that about 60% of human communication is nonverbal (Burgoon, 1985, Hall et al., 1995), it seems reasonable to assume that the interpretation of other's nonverbal behaviour may play a role in depressogenic interactions.

That non-verbal processes are involved in depressogenic interactions has been presumed by several authors (Argyle, 1978, Youngren and Lewinsohn, 1980, Gotlib and Robinson, 1982, Troisi et al., 1989, Bouhuys and Albersnagel, 1992, Segrin, 1993b, Segrin and Abramson, 1994, Bouhuys et al., 1995). Supportive evidence has for instance been found with respect to nonverbal behaviours that express involvement in an interaction. It has been demonstrated that the nonverbal expression of high involvement by patients and by the interviewing person predicted persistence of depression (Troisi et al., 1989Bouhuys and Albersnagel, 1992Bouhuys and Van den Hoofdakker, 1993Geerts et al., 1995). Moreover, Geerts et al. (1996)revealed that lack of attunement of such involvement behaviours between patients and a care-giver also predicted an unfavourable course of depression. They suggested that differences in information processing may underlie their findings. Hence, the appreciation of nonverbal signals is supposed to play a (causal) role in depression. In particular the face signals emotional states and regulates other's behaviour (Darwin, 1965, Ekman, 1992). Human facial displays, like all animal signals, are schematized for signal value in social interactions and reflect intentions of future behaviour, that are interpreted by the recipient. One may assume that correctly processing of emotional meaning of facial expressions is important for normal relationships. For other non-verbal behaviours such assumption has been proven. For instance, interactions between persons who both correctly interpret non-verbal behaviours are experienced as more meaningful and are accompanied with more mutual support (Hodgins and Zuckerman, 1990, Hall et al., 1995). We suggest that the negative bias in interpretation of facial expressions serves a mediating role in the negative course of interpersonal behaviour. Thus, negative interpretation of other's emotions may underlie the often reported rejection that depressives elicit in others (see Segrin, 1993a). In other words, negative interactions may be mediated by depressed patient's negative interpretations of other's facial expressions. We therefore feel that studying non-verbal emotion expression is relevant in research on depression onset and persistence. Most research on cognition and emotion has been performed on non-clinical populations using different versions of a Stroop task. In those designs putative mood-congruent stimuli are mostly negatively tuned words. We presumed that the appreciation of nonverbally expressed emotions would bear more relevance for depressogenic processes.

It is well known that anxiety often coincides with depression (Ormel et al., 1993, Pasnau and Bystritskt, 1994). In addition, high levels of co-occurring anxiety predict an unfavourable course of depression (Kupfer and Spiker, 1981McLeod et al., 1992Coryell et al., 1992Ball et al., 1994). In perceiving and interpreting facial expressions at least two cognitive processes are involved: attention and interpretation. In their review on cognitions and emotions, Mathews and McLeod (1994)concluded that there is relatively little evidence to suggest that elevated levels of depression are associated with the facilitative processing of emotionally negative information. On the other hand they found considerable empirical support for the hypothesis that such encoding bias is associated with elevated levels of anxiety. The same could be concluded for interpretation of information: An interpretation bias favouring negative meanings was related to anxiety rather than to depression.

One may argue that the negative attention and interpretation bias (sometimes) shown by depressed subjects might be mediated by co-occurring enhanced anxiety levels, that is, a negative bias is related with anxiety rather than with depression. Following this reasoning, variation in anxiety levels may explain two phenomena. First, it may account for the discrepancies found between studies on biases in perception of facial expressions. Evidence on negative biases in perception of facial expressions in depression is equivocal. Some authors found a depression related negative bias and others did not (respectively Mandal and Palchoudhury, 1985, Mandal and Bhattacharya, 1985, Gur et al., 1992, Rubinow and Post, 1992versus Walker et al., 1984, Zuroff and Colussy, 1986, Mandal, 1987, Archer et al., 1992, Gaebel and Woelwer, 1992, Bouhuys et al., 1996). Second, negative biases of high anxious depressed subjects may explain the often reported unfavourable course of depression in these subjects: The negative biases in the judgment of nonverbal behaviour may affect interpersonal processes negatively, resulting in persistence of depression.

Most research on cognition and emotion has been done on non-clinical populations, leaving unanswered the question as to whether in a clinically depressed population co-occurring anxiety may explain changes in perception. Moreover, as far as we know, the underlying assumption that anxiety-related cognitions may play a role in depression persistence has never been tested in a clinical population. The present study will go in more detail on these issues. We will investigate facial perception of negative and positive emotions in subjects with a primary depression differing in co-occurring anxiety levels. Moreover, we will investigate whether baseline perception of facial emotions is related with the subsequent course of depression, which may reveal some light on factors that determine depression course. In a longitudinal design, depression, anxiety and the perception of facial expressions will be assessed at admission (T0), and after 6 weeks (T1) and 30 weeks (T2) later.

The following results are anticipated:

  • 1.

    Levels of anxiety in clinically depressed patients are positively related with the perception of negative facial emotions: We anticipate that these relationships exist across subjects (at T0, T1, and T2) as well as within subjects (covariation of perception when anxiety and/or depression changes); No relationships between anxiety and the processing of positive facial expressions are expected.

  • 2.

    This negative bias is more strongly associated with levels of anxiety than with levels of depression.

  • 3.

    The putative anxiety-related negative bias predicts an unfavourable course of depression. If so, then it is more likely that differences in selective processing represent the cognitive substrate of vulnerability to emotional disorder persistence.

Section snippets

Subjects and design

Thirty-nine depressed patients were studied at admission of an out-patient clinic (T0), 6 weeks after admission (T1), and 30 weeks after admission (T2). Concerning other research questions, we reported on a subset of these patients earlier (Bouhuys et al., 1996).

Patients were included after they gave informed consent and had an initial severity of depression of at least 15 on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1961). The patients were diagnosed by an experienced clinical

Course of depression and anxiety

The mean severity of depression (BDI) at T0 (n=39) was 26.6±7.9 (SD) (range 15–52), and of anxiety T0 (n=39): 29.2±9.8 (SD) (range 14–59). Several patients did not fill out all follow-up questionnaires or failed to return them. The mean depression scores for the remaining patients were respectively at T1 (n=30): 21.6±10.1 (SD) (range 3–51) and at T2 (n=19): 16.7±12.5 (SD) (range 2–42). These scores were for anxiety: at T1 (n=23): 22.9±10.2 (SD) (range 11–47) and at T2 (n=18): 20.5±9.9 (SD)

Discussion

We postulated 1) that an anxiety-related mood-congruent bias with respect to the perception of facial expressions could be demonstrated in clinically depressed patients, and 2) that the perception of negative facial emotions would be associated with co-occurring anxiety levels rather than with depression, and 3) that this putative anxiety-related bias would predict subsequent course of depression. Such relationships would underscore the involvement of nonverbal interpersonal processes for

References (53)

  • American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, revised (DSM-III-R),...
  • Archer, J., Hay, D.C. and Young, A.W. (1992) Face processing in psychiatric conditions. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 31,...
  • Argyle, M. (1978) Non-verbal communication and mental disorder. Psychol. Med. 8,...
  • Arrindell, W.A. and Ettema, J.M. (1986) SCL-90 handleiding bij een multidimensionele psychopathologie indicator. Swets...
  • Ball, S.G., Otto, M.W., Pollack, M.H. and Rosenbaum, J.F. (1994) Predicting prospective episodes of depression in...
  • Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, T.E., Mock, J.E. and Erbaugh, J.K. (1961) An inventory for measuring depression....
  • Beck, A.T., Rush, A.J., Shaw, B.F. and Emery, G. (1979) Cognitive therapy of depression. John Wiley and Sons, Guilford...
  • Bouhuys, A.L., Bloem, G.M. and Groothuis, T.G.G. (1995) Induction of depressed and elated mood by music influences the...
  • Bouhuys, A.L., Geerts, E., Mersch, P.P. and Jenner, J.A. (1996) Nonverbal interpersonal sensitivity and persistence of...
  • Bouhuys, A.L. and Albersnagel, F.A. (1992) Do interactional capacities based on observed behaviour interfere with...
  • Bouhuys, A.L. and Van den Hoofdakker, R.H. (1993) A longitudinal study of interaction patterns of a psychiatrist and...
  • Burgoon, J.K. (1985) Nonverbal signals. In: M.L. Knapp and G.R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal communication,...
  • Clarke, G., Hops, H., Lewinsohn, P., Andrews, J., Seeley, J. and Williams, J. (1992) Cognitive-Behavioral Group...
  • Coryell, W., Endicott, J. and Winocur, G. (1992) Anxiety syndromes as epiphenomena of primary major depression: outcome...
  • Coyne, J.C., Burchill, S.A.L. and Stiles, W.B. (1990) An interactional perspective on depression. In: C.R. Snyder and...
  • Darwin, C. (1965) The expression of the emotions in man and animals (1872). University of Chicago Press, Chicago,...
  • Derogatis, L.R. (1973) SCL-90: an outpatient psychiatric rating scale—preliminary report. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 9,...
  • Ekman, P. (1992) Facial expressions of emotion: New findings, new questions. Psychol. Sci. 3(1),...
  • Ekman, P. and Oster, H. (1979) Facial expressions of emotion. Ann. Rev. of Psychol. 30,...
  • Gaebel, W. and Woelwer, W. (1992) Facial expression and emotional face recognition in schizophrenia and depression....
  • Geerts, E., Bouhuys, A.L., Meesters, Y. and Jansen, J.H.C. (1995) Observed behavior of patients with seasonal affective...
  • Geerts, E., Bouhuys, A.L. and Van den Hoofdakker, R.H. (1996) Nonverbal attunement between depressed patients and an...
  • Gotlib, I.H., McLachlan, A.L. and Katz, A.N. (1988) Biases in visual attention in depressed and nondepressed...
  • Gotlib, I.H. and Robinson, L.A. (1982) Responses to depressed individuals: discrepancies between self-report and...
  • Gotlib, I.H. and Hammen, C.L.(1992) Psychological aspects of depression; Towards a cognitive-interpersonal...
  • Gur, R.C., Erwin, R.J., Gur, R.E., Zwil, A.S., Heimberg, C. and Kraemer, H.C. (1992) Facial emotion discrimination: II....
  • Cited by (56)

    • Ruminative response style is associated with a negative bias in the perception of emotional facial expressions in healthy women without a history of clinical depression

      2019, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Participants received a financial compensation. Our perception of facial expressions (PFE) task consisted of 12 schematic oval faces (line drawings) as administered by Bouhuys et al. (1995, 1997). The facial expression line drawings were composed from one eyes and nose type, four eyebrow types and three mouth types (see Fig. 1).

    • Recognition of emotional facial expressions in anxious and nonanxious depression

      2016, Comprehensive Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although only a few studies have examined the role of anxiety symptoms in emotion recognition for individuals with MDD, initial evidence suggests that these symptoms may play a role in the negative recognition bias associated with depression. Bouhuys and colleagues (1997) found that increased anxiety in a depressed sample was associated with greater perception of negative emotions when viewing ambiguous facial stimuli [15]. Similarly, Suslow and colleagues (2004) found that individuals with MDD and a comorbid anxiety disorder were slower to respond to positive faces during a face-in-the-crowd task than those without comorbid anxiety, further suggesting diminished reactivity toward positive facial expressions [16].

    • Empathic behavioral and physiological responses to dynamic stimuli in depression

      2012, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      One line of research suggested emotion-specific abnormalities with a so-called ‘negativity bias’ (for a review see Bourke et al., 2010) accounting for ‘negative potentiation’ and ‘positive attenuation’ phenomena. This explains why depressed patients recognize sad facial expressions significantly better and show a larger attention bias towards them (‘negative potentiation’) (Gur et al., 1992; Matthews and Antes, 1992; Bouhuys et al., 1996; Hale, 1998; Elliot et al., 2002; Gotlib et al., 2004; Leppänen, 2006; Raes et al., 2006) but at the same time show reduced emotional reactions to happy facial expressions (‘positive attenuation’) (Sloan et al., 1997). Other studies showed that acute patients judged facial expressions more negatively than remitted patients and controls (e.g. Levkovitz et al., 2003).

    • Major depressive disorder skews the recognition of emotional prosody

      2011, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Archer et al., 1992; Feinberg et al., 1986; Gaebel and Wolwer, 1992; Mandal and Bhattacharya, 1985; Persad and Polivy, 1993; Zuroff and Colussy, 1986). The present results are thus more comparable to those of studies on the recognition of facial expressions using a methodology similar to ours, that is, by asking MDD patients to rate the intensity of different emotional states displayed in pictures of facial expressions (Bouhuys et al., 1995, 1999, 1997, 1996; Gur et al., 1992; Hale, 1998; Hale et al., 1998). These comparisons are of particular interest when it comes to explaining our present results.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text