Elsevier

Psychiatry Research

Volume 95, Issue 1, 24 July 2000, Pages 9-23
Psychiatry Research

A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(00)00162-1Get rights and content

Abstract

This study presents preliminary evidence of an association between polymorphic variation in the gene for monoamine oxidase-A (MAOA) and interindividual variability in aggressiveness, impulsivity and central nervous system (CNS) serotonergic responsivity. An apparently functional 30-bp VNTR in the promoter region of the X-chromosomal MAOA gene (MAOA-uVNTR), as well as a dinucleotide repeat in intron 2 (MAOA-CAn), was genotyped in a community sample of 110 men. All participants had completed standard interview and questionnaire measures of impulsivity, hostility and lifetime aggression history; in a majority of subjects (n=75), central serotonergic activity was also assessed by neuropsychopharmacologic challenge (prolactin response to fenfluramine hydrochloride). The four repeat variants of the MAOA-uVNTR polymorphism were grouped for analysis (alleles ‘1+ 4’ vs. ‘2+3’) based on prior evidence of enhanced transcriptional activity in MAOA promoter constructs with alleles 2 and 3 (repeats of intermediate length). Men in the 1/4 allele group scored significantly lower on a composite measure of dispositional aggressiveness and impulsivity (P<0.015) and showed more pronounced CNS serotonergic responsivity (P<0.02) than men in the 2/3 allele group. These associations were also significant on comparison of the more prevalent one and three alleles alone (encompassing 93% of subjects). Although in linkage disequilibrium with the MAOA-uVNTR polymorphism, MAOA-CAn repeat length variation did not vary significantly with respect to behavior or fenflluramine challenge in this sample. We conclude that the MAOA-uVNTR regulatory polymorphism may contribute, in part, to individual differences in both CNS serotonergic responsivity and personality traits germane to impulse control and antagonistic behavior.

Introduction

Serotonergic activity in the central nervous system (CNS) correlates negatively with aggressiveness, impulsivity and anger-related personality traits in diverse clinical, forensic and non-patient populations (e.g. Brown et al., 1979, Brown et al., 1982, Linnoila et al., 1983, Roy et al., 1988, Coccaro et al., 1989, Coccaro et al., 1995, Coccaro et al., 1997a, Coccaro et al., 1997b, Kruesi et al., 1990, Limson et al., 1991, O’Keane et al., 1992, Virkkunen et al., 1994, Cleare and Bond, 1997, Manuck et al., 1998). This generality of association suggests the possible influence of an underlying neurobehavioral dimension of individual differences. Such a dimension has been described by temperament theorists as a continuum of behavioral constraint, inhibition, or harm avoidance (Soubrié, 1986, Depue and Spoont, 1986, Cloninger, 1987, Gray, 1987, Zuckerman, 1995). Persons who exhibit ‘low’ serotonergic activity are thus thought to have a diminished capacity to restrain impulses, this disinhibition of otherwise constrained behavior reflecting, in turn, a disregard for future consequences, impaired learning, or insensitivity to cues for punishment. When conjoined with antagonistic motivation, reduced impulse control is experienced as impulsive aggression and expressed as irritability, acts of verbal aggression or physical assault, property destruction or other antisocial behavior. When self-injurious, impulsive aggression may predispose to suicide or attempted suicide, which are similarly associated with diminished serotonergic activity in clinical studies (e.g. Ågren, 1980, Rydin et al., 1982, Mann and Stanley, 1986, van Praag, 1986, Mann et al., 1992) and may be predicted from trait measures of aggressiveness and impulsivity (Mann et al., 1999).

Biologically mediated dimensions of behavioral variability have both genetic and environmental etiologies. Among twin studies of adult populations, for instance, psychometric indices of impulsivity and aggression show heritabilities of 0.30–0.70, with environmental contributions reflecting mainly ‘non-shared’ (idiosyncratic) developmental experiences (e.g. Rushton et al., 1986, Pedersen et al., 1988, Tellegen et al., 1988, Coccaro et al., 1993, Coccaro et al., 1997a, Coccaro et al., 1997b, Lyons et al., 1995, Seroczynski et al., 1999). Genetic influences on the phenotypic covariation of aggression and impulsivity measurements (that is, shared genetic effects) are also demonstrated in one recent study (Seroczynski et al., 1999). Although few investigators have directly addressed the heritability of CNS serotonergic activity (Oxenstierna et al., 1976, Meltzer and Arora, 1988, Higley et al., 1993), it is reported that first-degree relatives of personality-disordered (PD) patients who show diminished central serotonergic responsivity (indicated by a blunted prolactin response to the serotonergic agonist, fenfluramine) are more likely to have personality traits indicative of impulsive PD than the relatives of probands exhibiting larger prolactin responses (Coccaro et al., 1994b).

It is conceivable that such genetic influences stem from variation in genes coding for components of the serotonin system, including transmitter synthesis, release and reuptake, metabolism or receptor activation. We reported previously that individual differences in aggressive disposition and serotonergic function were associated with an intronic polymorphism of the gene for tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH), the rate-limiting enzyme in serotonin biosynthesis (Manuck et al., 1998). Other studies have also shown varying associations of intronic TPH polymorphisms with suicidality or hostility among diverse clinical samples (Nielsen et al., 1994, Buresi et al., 1997, Mann et al., 1997, New et al., 1998, Nielsen et al., 1998; but see also Bellivier et al., 1998, Furlong et al., 1998), although interpretation of these findings remains tentative due to dissimilarities of study population, methodology and outcome, as well as a lack of known functional variation in the TPH gene.

A second candidate gene germane to aggression/impulsivity is that coding for monoamine oxidase-A (MAOA), a degradative enzyme that preferentially catalyzes the deamination of serotonin and norepinephrine. Aggression in male transgenic mice is heightened by deletion of the MAOA gene (Cases et al., 1995) and a mutation in this gene may also be associated with human aggressive behavior. In particular, mild retardation and impulsive aggression (including arson and attempted rape) have been found to cosegregate among males of a large Dutch kindred with a chain termination mutation in the eighth exon of the MAOA gene (Brunner et al., 1993a, Brunner et al., 1993b). Although this mutation is rare and therefore not plausibly predictive of behavior in the general population, these results suggest that common allelic variation at the MAOA locus (Xp11.4–Xp11.3) might be associated with interindividual variability in impulsivity and aggressive disposition.

Recently, Sabol et al. (1998) described a variable repeat (VNTR) in the 5′-flanking region of the MAOA gene and demonstrated allele-specific variation in promotor activity in an in vitro assay system. In preliminary studies, this apparently functional length polymorphism was found to be associated with panic disorder among women (Deckert et al., 1999) and with antisocial PD in male alcoholics (Samochowiec et al., 1999). A second MAOA polymorphism (Black et al., 1991), an intronic dinucleotide CA repeat that is in linkage disequilibrium with the promotor-region MAOA polymorphism, however, was not found related to self-reported aggression or diagnosis of conduct disorder among adolescents (Vanyukov et al., 1995). The purpose of the present investigation was to further examine polymorphic variation in the MAOA gene and its possible association with aggression/impulsivity indices, as seen in an adult male, non-patient sample of community volunteers. As in our prior study of trait attributes associated with a polymorphism of the TPH gene (Manuck et al., 1999), subjects were derived from a separate investigation conducted to identify behavioral correlates of cardiovascular disease risk and risk factor modification. Several common measures of impulsiveness, hostility and aggression history were available from the parent study and, in a portion of the study sample, a fenfluramine challenge had been administered to assess individual differences in CNS serotonergic responsivity.

Section snippets

Subjects

Subjects were participants in the University of Pittsburgh Cholesterol and Risk Evaluation (CARE) project and were recruited via locally distributed brochures, posters and media advertisements. To simplify classification of MAOA genotypes, only men were studied here. Exclusion criteria included diagnoses of schizophrenia or delusional disorder, cancer, stroke, insulin-dependent diabetes, chronic kidney or liver disease, and use of psychotropic, glucocorticoid or hypolipidemic medication.

Results

In preliminary analyses, neither of the MAOA polymorphisms was associated with variability in participant age and marital or employment status. The MAOA-uVNTR 1/4 allele group reported slightly more education, however, than subjects of the 2/3 allele group [mean=16.6 (S.D.=2.7) vs. 15.4 (2.5) years, t108=2.3, P<0.03]; this difference was also seen on analysis of the one and three alleles only [mean=16.8 (2.8) vs. 15.4 (2.5) years, t100=2.5, P<0.015]. With respect to behavioral measurements (as

Discussion

In this study, participants having the 3- and 5-repeat variants of a 30-bp VNTR in the promoter region of the MAOA gene (allele group 1/4) scored modestly, but significantly, lower on a composite measure of dispositional aggressiveness and impulsivity than did subjects with 3.5 and 4 repeats of this sequence (allele group 2/3). These four alleles were grouped for analysis based on the observations of Sabol et al. (1998), who reported enhanced transcriptional efficiency for the 3.5- and 4-repeat

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by National Institutes of Health grants HL-48328 (MFM), HL-40962 (SBM) and MH-46745 (JJM).

References (84)

  • C. Konradi et al.

    Topographic immunocytochemical mapping of monoamine oxidase-A, monoamine oxidase-B and tyrosine hydroxylase in human postmortem brain stem

    Neuroscience

    (1988)
  • D. LeMarquand et al.

    Serotonin and alcohol intake, abuse, and dependence: clinical evidence

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • M. Linnoila et al.

    Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration differentiates impulsive from nonimpulsive violent behavior

    Life Sciences

    (1983)
  • A.K. Malhotra et al.

    Benefits and pitfalls encountered in psychiatric genetic association studies

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • S.B. Manuck et al.

    Aggression, impulsivity and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity in a nonpatient sample

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (1998)
  • S.B. Manuck et al.

    Aggression and anger-related traits associated with a polymorphism of the tryptophan hydroxylase gene

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • P.A. McBride et al.

    Effects of age and gender on CNS serotonergic responsivity in normal adults

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1990)
  • H.Y. Meltzer et al.

    Genetic control of serotonin uptake in blood platelets: a twin study

    Psychiatry Research

    (1988)
  • H.B. Moss et al.

    Serotonergic responsivity and behavioral dimensions in antisocial personality disorder with substance abuse

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1990)
  • A. Roy et al.

    Acting out hostility in normal volunteers: negative correlation with levels of 5HIAA in cerebrospinal fluid

    Psychiatry Research

    (1988)
  • E. Rydin et al.

    Rorschach ratings in depressed and suicidal patients with low levels of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in cerebrospinal fluid

    Psychiatry Research

    (1982)
  • J. Samochowiec et al.

    Association of a regulatory polymorphism in the promoter region of the monoamine oxidase A gene with antisocial alcoholism

    Psychiatry Research

    (1999)
  • A.L. Seroczynski et al.

    Etiology of the impulsivity/aggression relationship: genes or environment?

    Psychiatry Research

    (1999)
  • M.M. Vanyukov et al.

    A dinucleotide repeat polymorphism at the gene for monoamine oxidase A and measures of aggressiveness

    Psychiatry Research

    (1995)
  • D.B. Allison

    Transmission-disequilibrium tests for quantitative traits

    American Journal of Human Genetics

    (1997)
  • E.S. Barratt

    Impulsiveness subtraits: arousal and information processing

  • E.S. Barratt

    Impulsiveness and aggression

  • F. Bellivier et al.

    Association between the tryptophan hydroxylase gene and manic-depressive illness

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1998)
  • A.W. Bendig

    Factor analytic scales of covert and overt hostility

    Journal of Consulting Psychology

    (1962)
  • A.W. Biaggio

    Reliability and validity of four anger scales

    Journal of Personality Psychology

    (1981)
  • G.C.M. Black et al.

    Dinucleotide repeat polymorphism at the MAOA locus

    Nucleic Acids Research

    (1991)
  • J.M. Bland et al.

    Transformations, means, and confidence intervals

    British Medical Journal

    (1996)
  • E. Borroni et al.

    Differences between d-fenfluramine and d-norfenfluramine in serotonin presynaptic mechanisms

    Journal of Neurochemistry

    (1983)
  • G.L. Brown et al.

    Aggression, suicide and serotonin: Relationships to CSF amine metabolites

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (1982)
  • Brunner, H.G., 1996. MAOA deficiency and abnormal behavior: perspectives on an association. In: Ciba Foundation...
  • H.G. Brunner et al.

    Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monamine oxidase

    Science

    (1993)
  • H.G. Brunner et al.

    X-linked borderline mental retardation with prominent behavioral disturbance: phenotype, genetic localization, and evidence for disturbed monoamine metabolism

    American Journal of Human Genetics

    (1993)
  • C. Buresi et al.

    Association between suicide attempt and the tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH) gene

    American Journal of Human Genetics

    (1997)
  • A.H. Buss et al.

    An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility

    Journal of Consulting Psychology

    (1957)
  • O. Cases et al.

    Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA

    Science

    (1995)
  • C.R. Cloninger

    A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1987)
  • E.F. Coccaro

    Impulsive aggression and central serotonergic system function in humans. An example of a dimensional brain-behavior relationship

    International Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (1992)
  • Cited by (385)

    • The neurobiology of human aggressive behavior: Neuroimaging, genetic, and neurochemical aspects

      2021, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry
    • The role of monoamine oxidase A in the neurobiology of aggressive, antisocial, and violent behavior: A tale of mice and men

      2020, Progress in Neurobiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      While most research has linked the MAOA-L variant to the development of ASB with or without a history of childhood maltreatment, some investigations (typically based on smaller samples) have also shown a relationship between the MAOA-H alleles and antisocial and aggressive behavior. These studies have pointed to an association of the MAOA-H genetic variant with aggression among boys and adult men (Manuck et al., 2000; Brownlie et al., 2004). Additionally, an investigation of 174 male, Finnish violent offenders with an AUD (36 % had a diagnosis of ASPD) reported that the MAOA-H genetic variant was related to violent recidivism within eight years of follow-up among individuals with a history of childhood maltreatment.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text