Review
Animal cognition and the rat olfactory system

https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01625-9Get rights and content

Abstract

Is smell a ‘primitive’ sense used primarily to guide biologically basic behaviors or might it be the sensory modality that allows some species to express complex learning and other forms of cognitive behavior? Historically, the olfactory system has been considered primitive and it is not surprising that, until recently, cognitive neuroscientists have ignored odor-guided behavior. However, we now know that the olfactory system has projections to the prefrontal cortex, entorhinal cortex and hippocampus, and that these connections support the acquisition of simple and higher-order instrumental tasks, as well as a robust memory for odors. It appears that animals with a well-developed sense of smell have the neural machinery to think with their noses.

Section snippets

Learning sets, olfaction and cognition

In his classic studies on ‘learning set’, Harlow 6 showed that, over time, monkeys presented with hundreds of simple, two-object visual discrimination tasks gradually made fewer errors, eventually achieving nearly errorless learning. They did this by learning something about the rules of the game. The first trial of each new task provided information about which choice would be rewarded; errorless learning was revealed by a monkey's correct responses on subsequent trials. Adopting the rule

Olfactory matching-to-sample

In a matching-to-sample test an animal is given a sample stimulus and then one or more comparison stimuli (see Fig. 1). It is a potentially powerful method for assessing non-spatial working memory, the relationship between inter-item interference and short-term memory and other aspects of cognitive behavior. The subject's task is to choose the comparison stimulus that is identical or most similar to the sample. A brief delay before presenting the comparison stimuli can be used to assess

Memory for odors

Otto and Eichenbaum 15 used the non-matching-to-sample procedure to examine short-term odor memory by imposing a delay between sequential presentations of odors. Under optimal conditions (use of many odors and a short delay between odor presentations) non-matching-to-sample accuracy was 90%; this declined to 75% with a 60 s delay (Fig. 3). Considerably better short-term memory was demonstrated using a procedure that essentially eliminated interitem interference 16: after being trained on a

Odor paired-associate learning

Other odor-cued tasks further extend our appreciation of what can only be viewed as ‘higher order’ learning by rats. To study paired-associate learning, rats were trained to sample two successively presented odors and were rewarded if they responded only to certain pairs (the paired-associate odors) 20. Responses after presentations of other odor pairs were not reinforced. There were eight pairs of associate odors and 112 types of trial containing other (non-reinforced) odor combinations. Rats

Odors, transitive patterning and transitive inference

In a conditional discrimination task in which subjects are rewarded if they respond to only one of two simultaneously presented stimuli using stimulus pairs A+/B−, B+/C− and C+/A− (where+and – indicate the reinforced and non-reinforced stimuli, respectively, in each odor pair), the correct choice is dependent upon the configuration of the stimuli and not on associations between individual stimuli and reinforcement. Dusek and Eichenbaum 21 found that, with odor cues, rats readily learned this

Neurobiology of olfactory cognitive behavior

Two classes of neurobiological investigation have emerged from the rodent studies described above: (1) those that have addressed the question of which olfactory pathways mediate different types of olfactory learning; and (2) those using odor learning to examine more general issues in cognitive neuroscience. An example of the first approach is a study on the role of olfactory pathways in the acquisition of an olfactory learning set 25. Rats with lesions of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus (MD)

Conclusions

Comparative psychology is replete with demonstrations of small-brained animals, including insects, exhibiting complex associate learning and of larger-brained animals failing to learn similar tasks. As Dyer has observed, a major challenge in understanding animal cognition is to account for limitations in solving cognitive problems 35.

One such limitation may be unwittingly imposed when the experimenter uses stimulus cues that are inappropriate for efficient task performance. Most, and perhaps

Acknowledgements

I thank William Hodos and H. Philip Zeigler for their many useful suggestions and comments.

References (37)

  • P. Mobarts

    Visualizing an olfactory sensory map

    Cell

    (1996)
  • I.C. Reid et al.

    The enigma of olfactory learning

    Trends Neurosci.

    (1993)
  • B.M. Slotnick

    The enigma of olfactory learning revisited

    Neuroscience

    (1994)
  • T.P.S. Powell

    The central olfactory connections

    J. Anat.

    (1965)
  • J.L. Price et al.

    Dual olfactory representation in the rat thalamus: an anatomical and electrophysiological study

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1983)
  • W. Hodos

    Evolutionary interpretation of neural and behavioral studies of living vertebrates

  • S.B. Dunnett et al.

    Proactive interference effects on short-term memory in rats: I. basic parameters and drug effects

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (1990)
  • M.J. Pontecorvo

    Effects of proactive interference on rats’ continuous non-matching-to-sample performance

    Anim. Learning Behav.

    (1983)
  • Cited by (103)

    • Repeated reversals of concurrent olfactory discriminations in rats: A search for functional equivalence

      2021, Behavioural Processes
      Citation Excerpt :

      Rats in the present study completed a greater number of reversals (M = 29.3, Range = 12–60) than those in Dube et al. (M = 4.6, Range = 0–10), even though the number of sessions completed was comparable. This considerable difference in the number of reversals completed is likely due to the use of olfactory stimuli (see Nigrosh et al., 1975; Slotnick, 2001). However, the present study still had a relatively low number of reversals completed compared to Vaughan (1988) and Kastak et al. (2001).

    • Directional tuning in the hippocampal formation of birds

      2021, Current Biology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, the relative lack of place cells in our data and in data from other avian species remains a puzzle that is not likely to be accounted for by differences in behavior between birds and rodents, as here we recorded from birds freely roaming a square arena, similar in size and in recording conditions to many experiments in rats and mice that showed clear place cells and grid cells.50,87 Another possible explanation for the differences in spatial representation may stem from the fact that rodents strongly rely on somatosensation and olfaction,88,89 whereas quails and many other species of birds are primarily visual.90 Yet the findings of place cells in flying bats both when using vision and when using echolocation91 strongly suggest that, across species, the existence of place cells is not directly correlated with the dominant sensory modality of the species.

    • Rapidly training African giant pouched rats (Cricetomys ansorgei) with multiple targets for scent detection

      2020, Behavioural Processes
      Citation Excerpt :

      In Part 1, we compared methods to rapidly retrain rats with novel targets revealing that training could be expedited by relaxing the false alarm criterion early on and by introducing targets with only novel S-s. Thus, these procedures were adopted for all rats during Part 2. Based on the results from Part 1 and previous research on learning set effects (e.g., Slotnick, 2001; Williams and Johnston, 2002; Wright et al., 2016), we hypothesized the rats would learn all three targets in significantly less time than was required for Part 1, while maintaining the ability to find all trained odor targets during maintenance tests interleaved with novel target training. Rats received sequential training on three novel S + odors (Fig. 8) following the Experimental Procedures described in Part 1, unless otherwise noted.

    • Fear paradigms: The times they are a-changin’

      2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences
    • Successive odor matching- and non-matching-to-sample in rats: A reversal design

      2018, Behavioural Processes
      Citation Excerpt :

      The automated procedure increases experimental control by minimizing effects of handling and other distractions for the subject, as well as providing a more precise dependent measure. Slotnick and colleagues (see Slotnick, 2001 for a review) developed this procedure for rats and mice, demonstrating both discrimination and MTS with odor stimuli. In particular, Lu et al. (1993) used an automated procedure to test odor matching in rats, using successive conditional discrimination training (go, no-go).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text