Mindfulness and experiential avoidance as predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder avoidance symptom severity

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.02.005Get rights and content

Abstract

Mindfulness reflects an awareness of present moment experiences through an attitude of acceptance and openness (Bishop et al., 2004, Cardaciotto et al., 2008). Experiential avoidance, by contrast, refers to attempts to change, alter, or avoid private experiences (e.g., thoughts, feelings, sensations), and it is believed to underlie a number of psychopathologies, including PTSD (Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, & Strosahl, 1996). We were interested in the ability of mindfulness to predict the variance of PTSD avoidance symptom severity above and beyond experiential avoidance. 378 introductory psychology students were administered self-report measures of PTSD, mindfulness, experiential avoidance, thought suppression, alexithymia, and avoidant coping. Mindfulness, specifically nonjudgment of experiences, accounted for a unique portion of the variance in PTSD avoidance symptoms.

Introduction

Of the three symptom clusters associated with PTSD, research suggests that avoidance symptoms (criterion C) are the most reliable indicator that an individual may meet full PTSD criteria (see Nemeroff et al., 2006), and they appear to be most predictive of overall PTSD symptom severity (Boeschen et al., 2001, Marshall et al., 2006, Marx and Sloan, 2005). Criterion C symptoms include efforts to avoid experiences related to the trauma, difficulty recalling the trauma, diminished interest in activity, feelings of detachment, restricted affect, and a feeling that one's future has been foreshortened (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).

Avoidance strategies are also thought to underlie several psychopathologies, such as substance abuse and obsessive–compulsive disorder, as well as PTSD. These patterns of behaviors have been given the umbrella term experiential avoidance within the Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) literature (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999). Experiential avoidance occurs when an individual engages in strategies to blunt, alter, or control distressing private experiences, such as thoughts, emotions, and physiological sensations (Hayes et al., 1996). A reliance on experiential avoidance strategies appears to exacerbate or maintain PTSD symptoms over time (Tull, Gratz, Salters, & Roemer, 2004). Additionally, they may reduce one's flexibility in dealing with situations, negatively impacting quality of life (Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth, & Steger, 2006).

Following exposure to a traumatic event, avoidance behaviors may initially be focused on activities and stimuli that remind the individual of, or are connected to the individual's experience of the trauma; however, these strategies may gradually generalize to non-trauma related stimuli and contribute to the maintenance of posttraumatic symptoms in the long-term (Polusny and Follette, 1995, Rosenthal et al., 2005, Varra and Follette, 2005. Blackledge (2004) offers three reasons for why experiential avoidance may maintain PTSD symptoms over time: It limits opportunities for positive reinforcement; some types of avoidance behaviors such as substance abuse may increase exposure to aversive experiences; as no new learning occurs, it may maintain verbal rules which continue to limit exposure to real world consequences (e.g., “Being in crowds is dangerous”). As experiential avoidance is a very broad term, we were interested in examining specific avoidance strategies that have an established research base within the PTSD literature.

The term alexithymia was first introduced by Sifneos (1973) to describe patients exhibiting psychosomatic symptoms who had difficulty identifying and describing emotions. Recent research suggests that alexithymic individuals have difficulty drawing relationships between emotions and physiological sensations (Waller & Scheidt, 2004). Stewart, Zvolensky, and Eifert (2002) suggest that that alexithymia is a form of “emotional constriction” and subset of experiential avoidance.

The relationship between alexithymia and PTSD may be bidirectional. On one hand, there is evidence that individuals higher in alexithymia may be more likely to develop PTSD following trauma exposure (Kosten, Krystal, Giller, Frank, & Dan, 1992). Higher levels of alexithymia may also be a consequence of PTSD symptoms. Söndergaard and Theorell (2004) found that alexithymia increased following exposure to trauma in a sample of war refugees assessed at 3-month intervals. Badura (2003) suggests that alexithymia in individuals with PTSD may be better understood as representative of the emotional numbing component of PTSD than as a distinct construct, that individuals with PTSD begin to employ an avoidance-based coping style to deal with reexperiencing and hyperarousal symptoms. Other researchers have found a strong relationship between alexithymia and emotional numbing in individuals with PTSD (Frewen et al., 2008a, Frewen et al., 2008b, Fukunishi et al., 1996). Frewen et al. suggest that trauma disrupts an individual's ability to interpret the relationship between mind and body experiences.

Thought suppression is one of the most widely studied forms of experiential avoidance. There are three consequences associated with thought suppression: (1) an increased likelihood of target thoughts following suppression; (2) a sudden increase in target thoughts following suppression; (3) an increase in intrusive target thoughts during suppression when cognitive demands begin to interfere with attempts at suppression (Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000). The use of thought suppression appears to predict PTSD symptom severity (Mayou et al., 2002, Steil and Ehlers, 2000). In what they call the theory of ironic processes, Wenzlaff and Wegner (2000) argue that the continued monitoring for unwanted thoughts interferes with the overarching goal of thought suppression, making unwanted thoughts more salient and thus priming a rebound effect. Shipherd and Beck, 1999, Shipherd and Beck, 2005 have found that individuals with PTSD exhibited a rebound effect in trauma-related thoughts following a deliberate suppression, but trauma survivors without PTSD did not exhibit a rebound effect, suggesting that the rebound effect may play a role in the maintenance of PTSD symptoms.

Avoidant coping refers to the tendency to respond to distressing stimuli through distraction, such as through socializing or watching television (Endler & Parker, 1990, 1994). Following trauma, individuals may engage in strategies to avoid stimuli that remind them of the traumatic event (Steil & Ehlers, 2000). Although avoidant coping may be adaptive in the short term in some situations (Chaffin, Wherry, & Dykman, 1997), studies have found that an avoidant coping style is predictive of PTSD symptomatology (e.g., Gil, 2005, Scarpa et al., 2006). In the Gil study, the researcher had collected his sample of coping styles for another study 2 weeks prior to a terrorist attack and was able to collect a posttrauma measure of coping 1 month after the attack. PTSD was assessed 5 months later. Similar to alexithymia, the results of this research suggest a bidirectional relationship between avoidant coping and PTSD: Avoidant coping may create a vulnerability to developing PTSD following trauma exposure, and trauma survivors may begin applying avoidant coping strategies following trauma.

The concept of mindfulness was originally inspired by 2500 year-old Buddhist meditation practices. Much of the recent interest in mindfulness and mindfulness-based treatments can be traced to Kabat-Zinn's (1990) Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction program. Mindfulness techniques have been incorporated into several treatments associated with improved outcomes (e.g., Grossman et al., 2004, MAMIG, 2006).

As mindfulness-based treatments have proliferated, researchers have attempted to operationally define mindfulness. Definitions tend to include an awareness component and attitudinal component (e.g., Bishop et al., 2004, Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Most recently, Cardaciotto et al. (2008) have conceptualized mindfulness as a general tendency towards greater awareness of one's experiences, private and public, and bringing an attitude of acceptance and nonjudgment to these experiences. They suggest that awareness of experience and the attitude of acceptance with which one engages one's experience are orthogonal constructs. This makes the attitudinal component important in defining mindfulness, as awareness alone may be insufficient. Baer, Smith, and Allen (2004) found that in samples largely naïve to formal meditation practice, the tendency to be aware of one's experience was associated with greater judgment of that experience. Therefore, an attitude of openness and nonjudgment towards one's experiences may be a crucial mechanism of change in promoting the positive benefits associated with mindfulness (Shapiro, Carlson, Astin, & Freedman, 2006). This is consistent with an ACT definition of mindfulness, which includes contact with the present moment, acceptance of private experiences and willingness to remain in contact with them, and awareness of private experiences as content that are separate from the experience of one's self (Fletcher & Hayes, 2005). Within the ACT literature, acceptance of and willingness to embrace one's experience is reflective of a core ACT process that is considered the alternative to experiential avoidance (Hayes et al., 1999). Consequently, awareness of experience without acceptance may not rule out experiential avoidance.

Although individuals higher in mindfulness may experience negative thoughts, they appear to exhibit a greater ability to “let go” of negative thoughts and focus their attention on healthier ways of relating to their experiences (Frewen et al., 2008a). Greater mindfulness may enhance the ability of individuals to label negative affective stimuli, which may, in turn, allow them some degree of distance or detachment from these experiences (Creswell, Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007). Consequently, cultivation of mindfulness can create a sea change in the way one approaches one's experience, allowing one to develop greater stability, meaning, flexibility, and less reactivity (Shapiro et al., 2006).

The last ten years has seen an increase in the use of mindfulness-based treatments and techniques, in part to counter avoidance and help people engage their experiences more mindfully (see Baer, 2003). Although researchers have begun to explore the use of mindfulness in the treatment of trauma survivors (Batten et al., 2006, Follette et al., 2004), there has been little research exploring the relationship between mindfulness and PTSD; thus, the relationship between mindfulness and PTSD symptoms remains unclear.

The main purpose of this study was to incorporate mindfulness into an experiential avoidance understanding of PTSD. Relationships between measures of mindfulness and experiential avoidance have been explored in previous research (Baer et al., 2004, Baer et al., 2006, Hayes et al., 2004), but not within a trauma population. Understanding the relationship between mindfulness and PTSD could help researchers to tailor mindfulness and acceptance-based treatments for the treatment of trauma survivors. We were interested in exploring the relationship between experiential avoidance and PTSD avoidance symptoms, and whether mindfulness adds any additional predictive power. It was hypothesized that greater experiential avoidance would be predictive of greater PTSD avoidance symptoms severity, and that greater mindfulness would be predictive of lower PTSD avoidance symptom severity above and beyond measures of experiential avoidance.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants consisted of Introductory Psychology students (N = 378; 267 females), 18 years or older. (One demographics form was left incomplete.) Ages ranged from 18 to 51 (M = 19.56, SD = 3.44), with 18 being the modal age (45.2%) and 19 being the median age.

Measures

Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al., 2006). The FFMQ is a 39-item self-report measure of five facets of mindfulness derived from a factor analysis of five existing mindfulness measures. Items are rated on a Likert scale from

Internal consistency

The following Cronbach's coefficient alphas were computed for the entire sample. FFMQ subscales were: Observe (α = .77); describe (α = .90); act with awareness (α = .88); nonjudgment (α = .89); and nonreactivity (α = 75). Measures of experiential avoidance were: AAQ (α = .67); WBSI (α = .87); CISS emotion oriented coping (α = .88) and avoidance oriented coping (α = .80); TAS-20 subscales Factor 1 (identify feelings; α = .82), Factor 2 (describe feelings; α = .79), and Factor 3 (externally oriented thinking; α = .63).

Discussion

This study sought to investigate the degree to which measures of experiential avoidance predicted PTSD avoidance symptoms severity, and whether mindfulness adds to the prediction of PTSD avoidance symptoms, above and beyond the predictive ability of measures of experiential avoidance. This question was explored in a nonclinical sample of individuals who reported a Criterion A trauma on a self-report PTSD measure, but who did not meet full PTSD criteria according to the measure. We labeled this

Acknowledgments

This study was adapted from the first author's dissertation. He would like to thank David, Schuldberg, Gyda Swaney, Bryan Cochran, and Rita Sommers-Flanagan for their input.

References (64)

  • C.B. Nemeroff et al.

    Posttraumatic stress disorder: A state-of-the-art review

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (2006)
  • J.D. Parker et al.

    The 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale: III. Reliability and factorial validity in a community population

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    (2003)
  • M.A. Polusny et al.

    Long-term correlates of child sexual abuse: Theory and review of the empirical literature

    Applied & Preventive Psychology

    (1995)
  • J.C. Shipherd et al.

    The effects of suppressing trauma-related thoughts on women with rape-related posttraumatic stress disorder

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1999)
  • J.C. Shipherd et al.

    The role of thought suppression in posttraumatic stress disorder

    Behavior Therapy

    (2005)
  • R. Steil et al.

    Dysfunctional meaning of posttraumatic intrusions in chronic PTSD

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2000)
  • M.T. Tull et al.

    A preliminary investigation of the relationship between emotion regulation difficulties and posttraumatic stress symptoms

    Behavior Therapy

    (2007)
  • M.P. Twohig

    Acceptance and commitment therapy for treatment-resistant posttraumatic stress disorder: A case study

    Cognitive and Behavioral Practice

    (2009)
  • E. Waller et al.

    Somotoform disorders as disorders of affect regulation: A study comparing the TAS-20 with the non-self-report measures of alexithymia

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    (2004)
  • American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text...
  • R.A. Baer

    Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review

    Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice

    (2003)
  • R.A. Baer et al.

    Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills

    Assessment

    (2004)
  • R.A. Baer et al.

    Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness

    Assessment

    (2006)
  • R.A. Baer et al.

    Construct validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating samples

    Assessment

    (2008)
  • S.V. Batten et al.

    Acceptance and commitment therapy in the treatment of comorbid substance abuse and post-traumatic stress disorder: A case study

    Clinical Case Studies

    (2005)
  • S.V. Batten et al.

    Acceptance and mindfulness-based approaches to the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder

  • S.R. Bishop et al.

    Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition

    Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice

    (2004)
  • J.T. Blackledge

    Functional contextual processes in posttraumatic stress

    International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy

    (2004)
  • L.E. Boeschen et al.

    Experiential avoidance post-traumatic stress disorder: A cognitive mediational model of rape recovery

    Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma

    (2001)
  • R.A. Bryant et al.

    Maladaptive self-appraisals before trauma exposure predict posttraumatic stress disorder

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2007)
  • L. Cardaciotto et al.

    The assessment of present moment awareness and acceptance: The Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale

    Assessment

    (2008)
  • J.D. Creswell et al.

    Neural correlates of dispositional mindfulness during affect labeling

    Psychosomatic Medicine

    (2007)
  • Cited by (176)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text