Elsevier

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

Volume 47, November 2014, Pages 670-683
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

Review
The learning of fear extinction

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.016Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Recent papers on extinction learning are reviewed.

  • Brain regions involved in learning of fear extinction.

  • Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators that modulate fear extinction.

  • Suggestions about therapeutic applications of fear extinction.

Abstract

Recent work on the extinction of fear-motivated learning places emphasis on its putative circuitry and on its modulation. Extinction is the learned inhibition of retrieval of previously acquired responses. Fear extinction is used as a major component of exposure therapy in the treatment of fear memories such as those of the posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). It is initiated and maintained by interactions between the hippocampus, basolateral amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, which involve feedback regulation of the latter by the other two areas. Fear extinction depends on NMDA receptor activation. It is positively modulated by d-serine acting on the glycine site of NMDA receptors and blocked by AP5 (2-amino-5-phosphono propionate) in the three structures. In addition, histamine acting on H2 receptors and endocannabinoids acting on CB1 receptors in the three brain areas mentioned, and muscarinic cholinergic fibers from the medial septum to hippocampal CA1 positively modulate fear extinction. Importantly, fear extinction can be made state-dependent on circulating epinephrine, which may play a role in situations of stress. Exposure to a novel experience can strongly enhance the consolidation of fear extinction through a synaptic tagging and capture mechanism; this may be useful in the therapy of states caused by fear memory like PTSD.

Introduction

Extinction consists of the learned inhibition of retrieval of previously acquired memories. It was first described by Pavlov and Anrep (1927) in the early 1900s by systematically omitting the unconditioned stimulus (US, also called “reinforcement”) in test trials of a previously acquired conditioned reflex (CR). He studied this first in alimentary conditioning, where the conditioned stimulus (CS) was a sound and the US was a piece of meat, and then in what he called “defence conditioned reflexes”, one in which the US was the ingestion of acid, and another one in which the US was a shock to a hind leg. Defence conditioned reflexes are nowadays known by most neuroscientists as forms of “learned fear”. Pavlov and his original followers did not use the term “fear” because it implies assuming that the observed behavior of animals is equal to the complex phenomenon that humans call ‘fear”, which englobes subjective components, some of which are conscious and others constitute a particular unconscious state. There is no way of knowing whether animals experience a similar state and a conscious realization of threats as humans do. This can only be inferred from interpretations of animal behaviors in human terms, by analogy. One of the most influential workers in the field, Joseph LeDoux, suggests that the terms “threat” and “defense responses” should be used instead of “fear memory” or “fear responses” (LeDoux, 2014, Schiller et al., 2013). He realizes, like Pavlov, that the mechanisms through which the brain responds to threats are “distinct from those that make possible the conscious feeling of fear that can occur when one is in danger” (LeDoux, 2014). We can infer from the behavior of animals that they do perceive and recognize threats in “defence conditioned reflex situations”: to begin with, they tend to escape. But we cannot deduce that they experience or “feel” exactly what humans call “fear”.

In spite of agreeing with the point of view of Pavlov or LeDoux, we will refer throughout this article to “fear conditioning”, “fear memory” and “fear extinction”, just because it is more familiar to a majority of workers in the field and thus easily understandable by all of them; but we will not refer to “fear mechanisms” because these may be different in humans and in laboratory animals. There are many metaphorical terms in Biology and especially in the health sciences, which are customarily used despite their real or original meaning, like “anemia”. This word has been used for centuries to express a low level of oxygenated hemoglobin, although the word comes from the Greek a haima, which means, “lack of blood” or “no blood”. The term “fear” as applied to animals other than humans is also a metaphor coming from what we humans experience in threat situations. There is no way of knowing if animals exposed to a threat “feel” the peculiar combination of states that humans call “fear”. In particular, aside from the conscious feeling of fear that varies from species to species, the accompanying unconscious state of fear is difficult to define, and persists during and in spite of extinction (Costanzi et al., 2011). It may be different in humans and in laboratory animals (LeDoux, 2014). In rats, it has been called “drive” and defined as a nonassociative entity responsible for pseudoconditioned responses that “contaminate” real learned responding (Izquierdo and Cavalheiro, 1976a; see Wyrwicka, 1999). This unconscious state is probably at the root of the known fact that sudden unexpected stimuli may recover the original task long after it has been completely extinguished (Maren, 2014).

This state may be reconstructed or rekindled by retrieval, and we think it might be related to the phenomenon or process of reconsolidation described in recent years (Nader et al., 2000, Nader, 2003), which has changed our outlook on memory processes quite a lot.

Reconsolidation develops following retrieval in parallel to extinction and also necessitates ribosomal (Nader et al., 2000, Duvarci et al., 2008) and nonribosomal (Myskiw et al., 2008) protein synthesis in the hippocampus (Nader et al., 2000, Duvarci et al., 2008) and, as described initially (Nader et al., 2000), in the amygdala (Duvarci et al., 2008). It is a process whose probable main raison-d’être is to update (add information to, or change the meaning of) memories (Sara, 2000a, Sara, 2000b, Schiller et al., 2010, Forcato et al., 2010, Forcato et al., 2013). As time passes, and the interval between training and the first session of retrieval becomes longer, the probability of extinction predominates over that of reconsolidation, which typically can be seen only at relatively short training-retrieval intervals (a few days at the most; see Milekic and Alberini, 2002, Inda et al., 2011). The reconsolidation of memories motivated by alimentary or other reinforcers unrelated to fear has been much less studied than that of fear memories, even less than the extinction of alimentary conditioning. There have been, however, some exceptions, like the reconsolidation of object recognition in rats (Myskiw et al., 2008) and declarative verbal learning in humans (Forcato et al., 2010, Forcato et al., 2013).

In the first 30 years after its discovery, the study of extinction was restricted to classical conditioning, in which there is a pairing of the CS and the US regardless of the performance of CRs. Instrumental conditioning in which the CR is used by the animals as an instrument to either get or avoid the US was discovered in Pavlov's laboratory only in 1937 (Konorski and Miller, 1937). In the same year, its mechanical version called operant conditioning was first described independently in Minnesota by Skinner (1937); see Wyrwicka (1994). We prefer the term “instrumental” to “operant” because except in specially designed apparatuses the responses of the animals do not directly “operate” any gadget: freezing, flexing a leg, crossing a hurdle, omitting a response, salivating, etc. are the most commonly studied instrumental responses in fear motivated tasks. The animals use the response as an instrument to either obtain the US (food, water) in alimentary tasks or to prevent delivery of the US (usually a footshock) in fear-motivated tasks. The term “operant” is still widely used in the U.S.A. to denote instrumental conditioning because of the Skinnerian tradition.

As said, in classical conditioning the development of CRs depends on the pairing of an initially neutral stimulus with the US (Izquierdo and Cavalheiro, 1976a); through this pairing the neutral stimulus then becomes a conditioned stimulus (CS). Classical fear conditioning still is the most widely used animal model to study fear-motivated learning (or defensive responses to threats). It is acquired quickly, lasts very long, and is amenable to physiological, pharmacological and behavioral observations. Most of what we know today about the brain's fear system and its modulation comes from research using Pavlovian fear conditioning.

The development of CRs in instrumental conditioning depends not on the CS–US pairing but rather on the contingency between the CR and the US (Izquierdo and Cavalheiro, 1976a, Izquierdo and Cavalheiro, 1976b). The performance of a given CR (e.g., leg flexion) determines whether the animals will receive the following US (e.g., a footshock). In fear-motivated tasks, which have been by far the most used forms of instrumental conditioning in the last 50 years (Gold, 1986, Izquierdo and Medina, 1997, Izquierdo et al., 2006, LeDoux, 2014, De Quervain and McGaugh, 2014), the contingency may rely on the performance of a given CR or in the inhibition of a response in order to avoid a US; for example, animals may be taught to cross a line or to jump or not to cross a line or to refrain from jumping in order to avoid a footshock; i.e., they may learn to emit or to omit a response. Fear- or displeasure-motivated instrumental learning is also called “avoidance” learning: the CR is used to effectively avoid the US.

The study of extinction in instrumental (avoidance) conditioning started with Konorski and Miller (1937) and its analysis in forms of learning other than conditioned reflexes began years later. It was clear from the beginning that fear extinction in instrumental conditioning is slower than in classical conditioning, probably because in the former animals are trained in the initial task using partial schedules of reinforcement due to the relation that is to be acquired between performance of the CR and delivery of the US (Izquierdo et al., 1965).

The systematic omission of the reinforcement regardless of responses results in an inhibition of performance of the CR, which Pavlov and Anrep (1927) called experimental extinction or just extinction. The name and the definition persist till today (Myskiw et al., 2014, Izquierdo et al., 2014, De Quervain and McGaugh, 2014, LeDoux, 2014). Years after its original discovery by Pavlov, extinction was studied in detail by Konorski, 1948, Konorski, 1967, Rescorla, 2001, Rescorla, 2004 and many others, each of whom has contributed with factual and theoretical data (see Izquierdo et al., 1965, Izquierdo et al., 2014, Morgan et al., 1993, Bouton and Ricker, 1994, Bouton, 2004, Wyrwicka, 1999, Myers and Davis, 2002, Myers and Davis, 2007, Lattal et al., 2006, Davis, 2011, Maren, 2011, Milad and Quirk, 2012, Orsini and Maren, 2012, De Carvalho Myskiw et al., 2013, De Carvalho Myskiw et al., 2014; for references).

Some have pointed to the similarities between the physiological bases of extinction and those of conditioning or other forms of learning (Konorski, 1948, Konorski, 1967, Vianna et al., 2000, Vianna et al., 2001), while others pointed to the differences (see references in Lattal et al., 2006, Orsini and Maren, 2012). Since the days of Pavlov, most extinction experiments use some form of conditioning as the initial learning task (see Morgan et al., 1993, Lattal et al., 2006, Myers and Davis, 2007, Davis, 2011, Orsini and Maren, 2012, Milad and Quirk, 2012, Rosa et al., 2013, De Carvalho Myskiw et al., 2013, Izquierdo et al., 2014), because it makes the inhibition of the previously learned response more obvious than other procedures: first a CR has to be established and then it has to be inhibited.

There is now consensus that there are both molecular and physiological similarities and differences between extinction and all other forms of learning. The need for glutamate NMDA receptor-mediated plasticity [long-term potentiation (LTP) or long-term depression (LTD)] in hippocampus, basolateral amygdala (BLA) and/or ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) (Fiorenza et al., 2012, Orsini and Maren, 2012, Lemon and Manahan-Vaughan, 2012), and the need of posttraining protein synthesis for memory consolidation in these same areas (Vianna et al., 2001, Vianna et al., 2004, Santini et al., 2001) or in others (Berman and Dudai, 2001) are two key similarities between extinction and other forms of learning. Putative or observed actin cytoskeletal rearrangements (Fischer et al., 2004, Lattal et al., 2006) and synaptic growth or morphological alteration, including disappearance (Greenough, 2000, Geinisman, 2000, Ganeshina et al., 2004) have been described both in NMDA-mediated plasticity in hippocampus and in a variety of learning tasks in hippocampus and elsewhere. Indeed, some of the changes produced by exposure therapy (extinction) in humans can indeed be very long-lasting as assessed by fMRI studies (Hauner et al., 2012), which suggests morphological and functional effects.

Section snippets

Clinical use of fear extinction

Extinction learning is the basis of the widely used “exposure” psychotherapy of fear memories, especially those underlying posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Milad and Quirk, 2012, Sher and Vilens, 2010, Davis, 2011). In exposure therapy, subjects are exposed to stimuli related to those that led to a traumatic experience until they suppress inadequate responses to fear and they become able to carry a normal life; people with PTSD often recall their traumatic experiences repeatedly, intensely

Extinction is not forgetting

Some, even within Neuroscience, have confused extinction with forgetting (Tsumoto, 1990). Extinction may look like forgetting because a given stimulus or constellation of stimuli that yielded a consistent learned response becomes at some point unable to do so. It can, in theory, also eventually lead to forgetting by the disuse of the neurons involved in a given response (e.g., Eccles, 1964, Quirk et al., 2010). However, there are several reasons why extinction is viewed as one more form of

Properties of extinction showing that it consists in the inhibition of retrieval

Several fundamental properties of extinction show that it does not consist of erasure or attenuation of the initial learning. One is spontaneous recovery, which is brought about by an increased interval between extinction trials. This property, like extinction itself, was described first for alimentary conditioning (Pavlov and Anrep, 1927). Another property of extinction that shows that it is not equal to forgetting is renewal, which consists of its enhancement by a change of context (Bouton

A note on habituation learning

Habituation is a form of inhibitory learning in a way akin to extinction: it also consists of learning not to respond, but instead of not responding to a CS, subjects learn not to respond to normally neutral and inoffensive stimuli with orienting or exploratory responses, i.e. not to perform what Pavlov called the “what is it?” reflex. It was also first described by Pavlov and Anrep (1927), who viewed habituation and extinction as forms of “internal inhibition”, in contrast to the “external

Different brain regions involved in extinction learning and their relevant connections

A common belief over the years has been that the brain areas involved in acquisition must also be involved in the extinction of the diverse tasks. While this may partly be true in some cases, it is not in the vast majority of cases.

This belief probably resulted from pre-neuroscientific readings of the ideas of Pavlov's group expressed more than a hundred years ago that behaviors result from the irradiation of excitation or inhibition from the locus of a “temporary connection”, or may fade as a

Extinction and NMDA receptors

Perhaps the most direct way to affect extinction pharmacologically is by agents acting on the glutamate NMDA receptors involved in its generation. The first experimental postulation of a role of these receptors in the extinction of fear or fear-related learning was in a study carried out by Davis and his coworkers in which they found that the selective antagonist of this receptor, AP5 (d-amino-2-phosphono-pentanoic acid) given into the amygdala of rats blocked extinction of a conditioned

Histamine modulation of fear extinction learning

The first description of post-training enhancement of memory consolidation by histamine was by De Almeida and Izquierdo, 1986 using infusions of low doses of this drug into the lateral ventricles of rats in a fear conditioning paradigm. The well-known mast cell histamine releaser, 48/80 was without effect on memory when given intracerebroventricularly (De Almeida and Izquierdo, 1986). The results were clear and suggested a role for endogenous brain tissue but not mast cell histamine, but were

Endocannabinoid modulation of fear learning

Giovanni Marsicano and his coworkers first described and then systematically studied for more than 20 years the modulation of fear extinction (Marsicano et al., 2002). To summarize their extensive studies, they have clearly shown that the endogenous cannabinoid agonist, anandamide and the CB1 receptor positively modulate fear extinction through an action mainly in hippocampus and BLA, and, as they recently found, also in vmPFC (Do Monte et al., 2013a).

One possible mechanism of action of this

Other neurotransmitters and neuromodulators that modulate fear extinction

Dopamine acting on D1 or D2 receptors, noradrenaline acting on β adrenoreceptors (Berlau and McGaugh, 2006, Mueller et al., 2010, Fiorenza et al., 2012), the glucocorticoids which might lay an endogenous role in the modulation of extinction of stressful tasks (Roozendaal and McGaugh, 2011, De Quervain and McGaugh, 2014), the brain derived neurotrophic factor, BDNF (Peters et al., 2010, Do Monte et al., 2013a, Do Monte et al., 2013b) and, as mentioned above, cholinergic muscarinic mechanisms in

State-dependency of extinction learning

Many substances may modify memory, either when given prior to or after training. In some cases, they may actually be incorporated to the tasks as part of the constellation of CSs, and then act as retrieval cues (Overton, 1966, Overton, 1978). This is called state-dependency: memory becomes dependent of state(s) induced by the drugs. When these substances are endogenous, the process is called endogenous state-dependency (Zornetzer, 1978, Izquierdo and Dias, 1983, Izquierdo, 1984, Rosa et al.,

The synaptic (behavioral) tagging of extinction learning

Frey and Morris, 1997, Frey and Morris, 1998 and their associates (Sajikumar and Frey, 2004, Uzakov et al., 2005, Frey and Frey, 2008, Almaguer-Melian et al., 2010, Almaguer-Melian et al., 2012, Redondo and Morris, 2011) proposed a hippocampal mechanism through which hippocampal plastic events (LTP, LTD) would promote the synthesis of new proteins that can “tag” the synapses that had been activated and “capture” other plasticity-related proteins (PRPs) produced at other synapses by other LTPs

Extinction and addiction

Addictive drugs have strong effects on memory (Izquierdo and McGaugh, 1995). Some view addiction itself as a form of learning, based on solid evidence (Robbins, 1995, Hyman and Malenka, 2001, Hyman et al., 2006). The strategies and treatments used to overcome addiction certainly involve learning to avoid the drug(s) of abuse, including alcohol, by a learning process that focuses on the cessation of their intake based on the fear of falling back into patterns of abuse and addiction. Thus, it may

A final comment

There still are many aspects of the physiology of fear extinction that remain to be studied, there have been great advances in the last 5 or 10 years. Although with lingering doubts, several aspects of its circuitry are now known. Perhaps the main endogenous modulatory factors are known, namely histamine, endocannabinoids and the glucocorticoids. State dependency of extinction at least on peripheral epinephrine levels such as are known to increase in stress has been described and may be an

Acknowledgement

Work supported by the National Research Council of Brazil (CNPq).

References (245)

  • S.A. Carmack et al.

    The competitive NMDA receptor antagonist CPP disrupts cocaine-induced conditioned place preference, but spares behavioral sensitization

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2013)
  • V. Chevaleyre et al.

    Heterosynaptic LTD of hippocampal GABAergic synapses: a novel role of endocannabinoids in regulating excitability

    Neuron

    (2003)
  • E.C. Clayton et al.

    Adrenergic activation of the nucleus tractus solitarius potentiates amygdala norepinephrine release and enhances retention performance in emotionally arousing and spatial memory tasks

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • E.C. Clayton et al.

    Posttraining inactivation of excitatory afferent input to the locus coeruleus impairs retention in an inhibitory avoidance learning task

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (2000)
  • C.K.B. Da Silveira et al.

    The role of histamine receptors in the consolidation of object recognition memory

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (2013)
  • G.L. Dalton et al.

    NMDA GluN2A and GluN2B receptors play separate roles in the induction of LTP and LTD in the amygdala and in the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear

    Neuropharmacology

    (2012)
  • M. Davis et al.

    Effects of d-cycloserine on extinction: translation from preclinical to clinical work

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • M.R. Delgado et al.

    Neural circuitry underlying the regulation of conditioned fear and its relation to extinction

    Neuron

    (2008)
  • M.R. Delgado et al.

    Extending animal models of fear conditioning to humans

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2006)
  • F.H. Do Monte et al.

    Infusion of cannabidiol into infralimbic cortex facilitates fear extinction via CB1 receptors

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2013)
  • N.G. Fiorenza et al.

    Modulation of the extinction of two different fear-motivated tasks in three distinct brain areas

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2012)
  • C. Forcato et al.

    Reconsolidation in humans opens up declarative memory to the entrance of new information

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (2010)
  • S. Frey et al.

    Synaptic tagging” and “cross-tagging” and related associative reinforcement processes of functional plasticity as the cellular basis for memory formation

    Prog. Brain Res.

    (2008)
  • U. Frey et al.

    Synaptic tagging: implications for late maintenance of hippocampal long-term potentiation

    Trends Neurosci.

    (1998)
  • O. Ganeshina et al.

    Synapses with a segmented, completely partitioned postsynaptic density express more AMPA receptors than other axospinous synaptic junctions

    Neuroscience

    (2004)
  • J.C. Gewirtz et al.

    Is the hippocampus necessary for contextual fear conditioning?

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • M. Ghiasvand et al.

    β1-noradrenergic system of the central amygdala is involved in state-dependent memory induced by a cannabinoid agonist, WIN55,212-2, in rat

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2011)
  • P.E. Gold

    The use of avoidance training in studies of modulation of memory storage

    Behav. Neural Biol.

    (1986)
  • P.E. Gold et al.

    The mnemon and its juices: neuromodulation of memory processes

    Behav. Neural Biol.

    (1983)
  • I. Goshen et al.

    Dynamics of retrieval strategies for remote memories

    Cell

    (2011)
  • A.L. Guedea et al.

    ERK-associated changes of AP-1 proteins during fear extinction

    Mol. Cell. Neurosci.

    (2011)
  • R. Hirsh

    The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: a theory

    Behav. Biol.

    (1974)
  • R. Hirsh et al.

    Hippocampal mossy fibers, motivational states, and contextual retrieval

    Exp. Neurol.

    (1978)
  • A. Holmes et al.

    Pharmacological facilitation of fear extinction and the search for adjunct treatments for anxiety disorders—the case of yohimbine

    Trends Pharmacol. Sci.

    (2010)
  • M. Horio et al.

    Is d-cycloserine a prodrug for d-serine in the brain?

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2013)
  • H. Abush et al.

    Cannabinoids modulate hippocampal memory and plasticity

    Hippocampus

    (2010)
  • R. Adolphs et al.

    Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans

    Learn. Mem. Cold Spring Harb. N.Y.

    (1997)
  • W. Almaguer-Melian et al.

    Novelty exposure overcomes foot shock-induced spatial-memory impairment by processes of synaptic-tagging in rats

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (2012)
  • J.C. Ashton et al.

    The effect of delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol on the extinction of an adverse associative memory

    Pharmacology

    (2008)
  • S.C. Azad et al.

    Activation of CB1 specifically located on GABAergic interneurons inhibits LTD in the lateral amygdala

    Learn. Mem. Cold Spring Harb. N.Y.

    (2008)
  • F. Ballarini et al.

    Behavioral tagging is a general mechanism of long-term memory formation

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (2009)
  • F. Benetti et al.

    Histaminergic ligands injected into the nucleus basalis magnocellularis differentially affect fear conditioning consolidation

    Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol.

    (2013)
  • F. Benetti et al.

    Histamine infused into basolateral amygdala enhances memory consolidation of inhibitory avoidance

    Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol.

    (2013)
  • M.M. Bergamaschi et al.

    Cannabidiol reduces the anxiety induced by simulated public speaking in treatment-naïve social phobia patients

    Neuropsychopharmacology

    (2011)
  • D.E. Berman et al.

    Memory extinction, learning anew, and learning the new: dissociations in the molecular machinery of learning in cortex

    Science

    (2001)
  • R. Bernabeu et al.

    Involvement of hippocampal cAMP/cAMP-dependent protein kinase signaling pathways in a late memory consolidation phase of aversively motivated learning in rats

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (1997)
  • R. Bernabeu et al.

    Hippocampal cGMP and cAMP are differentially involved in memory processing of inhibitory avoidance learning

    Neuroreport

    (1996)
  • P. Blandina et al.

    Histamine neurons in the tuberomamillary nucleus: a whole center or distinct subpopulations?

    Front. Syst. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • J.S. Bonini et al.

    Histamine facilitates consolidation of fear extinction

    Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol.

    (2011)
  • M.E. Bouton

    Context and behavioral processes in extinction

    Learn. Mem. Cold Spring Harb. N.Y.

    (2004)
  • Cited by (95)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text