A deficit in discriminating gaze direction in a case with right superior temporal gyrus lesion

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.018Get rights and content

Abstract

The superior temporal sulcus (STS) region is well recognized as being heavily involved in detecting and discriminating gaze. Lesions confined to this area are quite rare in humans, and so the research has mainly depended on animal studies and functional neuroimaging in normal human subjects. We report one such rare case, a 54-year-old Japanese female with a possible congenital s anomaly who, after a cerebral hemorrhage, demonstrated a lesion almost completely confined to the entire right superior temporal gyrus (STG). In the subacute phase, the patient showed evidence of left hemispatial neglect, from which she gradually recovered. In the chronic phase, she showed a puzzling difficulty in obtaining eye-contact. We have conducted, in conjunction with conventional neuropsychological evaluations, experimental assessment of her ability in gaze cognition. Her performance on neuropsychological testing demonstrated no compromise in intellect, memory, or language skills, and a close-to-full recovery from neglect. On gaze cognition experiments, she was repeatedly shown to perceive left gaze as straight, and to a lesser degree, straight gaze as right. We suggest that the function of the STG in detecting gaze, together with the directional information it receives from earlier visual areas, may be associated, when damaged, with this deficit in detecting contra-directional gaze. We have demonstrated for the first time that a single circumscribed lesion to the STG results in both gaze processing deficit and concurrent aberrant gaze behavior of the victim herself, implicating a mechanism within the STG as an interface between gaze of others and gaze of self.

Introduction

The superior temporal sulcus (STS) has attracted much interest due to its intriguing function as a detector of biological motion. Since the 1980s, single-cell studies in monkey brains have shown that a number of neurons in the STS region show preference in the direction of head and gaze when viewing faces of conspecifics and humans, so that some neurons respond maximally to straight gaze while others respond more to averted gaze (Perrett, Hietanen, Oram, & Benson, 1992). Investigations of STS-lesioned monkeys showed disrupted gaze discrimination after lesioning (Campbell, Heywood, Cowey, Regard, & Landis, 1990; Eacott, Heywood, Gross, & Cowey, 1993), more or less supporting the findings from single-cell studies. Research of the STS in humans has found a homologous function; gaze as well as head direction, movement of mouth and hands, body gestures, geometric figures simulating biological motion have all been shown to activate the STS in functional imaging studies and electrophysiological studies (see Allison, Puce, & McCarthy, 2000; Puce & Perrett, 2003 for review). Thus, the function of the STS in humans can be summarized as the detection of biological motion, which composes a major contribution to social perception.

Gaze, in particular, is undoubtedly special, perhaps without a match in its significance as a biological signal. There are suggestions for the innateness of gaze cognition, in that newborns show a preference for faces with eyes open versus eyes closed (Batki, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Connellan, & Ahluwalia, 2000), and that infants as early as 10 weeks of age follow the gaze of others (Hood, Willen, & Driver, 1998). Moreover, children as young as 4–5 years of age show much more organized ERPs (N190) to eyes compared to full faces (Taylor, Edmonds, McCarthy, & Allison, 2001a), implicating a system within the human brain which is dedicated, at least in part, to gaze cognition. Such a system has been suggested to be localized in the posterior region of the STS in question, particularly in the right hemisphere: In a PET study, faces with visible gaze compared with obscured gaze, was shown to differentially activate the STS (Wicker, Michel, Henaff, & Decety, 1998). Gaze processing, such as perception of apparent eye motion (Puce, Allison, Bentin, Gore, & McCarthy, 1998) and detection of gaze direction (Hoffman & Haxby, 2000), has also been demonstrated to elicit significant STS activation in fMRI studies. ERP studies have shown that N170 from the posterior temporal region is also sensitive to gaze (Puce, Smith, & Allison, 2000; Taylor, Itier, Allison, & Edmonds, 2001b).

Intriguing as it may be, however, the function of the STS in humans remains tentative in that most studies are based on functional findings of the intact brain. Neuropsychological studies of human cases with circumscribed STS lesions might be more conclusive, and would afford a wealth of insight into these issues. Since left STS damage would leave most humans aphasic with little understanding of any verbal input, study of a patient with right STS damage is likely to be more informative. To date, and to our knowledge, such a neuropsychological case has never been reported. In this paper, we report one such case, who, after a cerebrovascular accident, presented a very rare circumscribed lesion almost confined to the entire right superior temporal gyrus (STG), which constitutes the upper bank of the STS. The rarity of this lesion might have partly been due to a co-existing cerebrovascular anomaly in this patient. One of the most striking features of the sequelae in this patient was the inability to engage in mutual eye-contact. Given the functional data suggesting STS involvement in gaze cognition, a quantitative evaluation of the patient's gaze processing would offer the first neuropsychological data for the STS and its suggested role in gaze cognition. We have therefore conducted, in addition to conventional neuropsychological assessments, some behavioral experiments in gaze processing which revealed a unique impairment in gaze cognition.

Section snippets

Case description

The patient, M.J. is a right-handed, Japanese female with high school education who has been married for over 30 years and has successfully reared two children. She has no prior neurological or psychiatric history. At the time of onset, she was 54 years old. She presented with headache, nausea, slight left hemiparesis, and clouded consciousness. She was transferred to a nearby hospital, where her cranial CT revealed a high density area in the right temporal lobe with a midline shift to the

Neuropsychological assessment

Assessment was performed with conventional neuropsychological tests on two occasions (Table 1). The first assessment was administered 2–4 months after the onset of the hemorrhage when M.J. still had dense hemispatial neglect. The second assessment was delivered a year after the first, when the neglect had remitted. Between these two assessments, M.J. visited the outpatient department twice a month for non-structured rehabilitation.

Experiment 1: gaze direction discrimination in digitized photographs

This and the following experimental investigations were carried out from the time of the second neuropsychological assessment, for a period of about a year. M.J. gave informed consent to participation in all experimental investigations. This study was approved by the ethical committee at our institutions. Note that all indications of ‘right’ and ‘left’ will refer to the direction from M.J.'s viewpoint. Note also that all materials were presented in her midsagittal plane, and that she was free

Experiment 2: mutual gaze detection

Given the suggestion that mutual gaze might be significant in its own right (George, Driver, & Dolan, 2001; Hooker et al., 2003, Kampe et al., 2001; Kawashima et al., 1999), we wanted to test whether M.J. had preserved cognition of this fundamental aspect of gaze—he detection of mutual gaze. We realized that neither photographs nor videotapes would be a true equivalent of a real-life mutual gaze, so we designed this experiment with real-life gaze in a more natural setting.

Experiment 3: direction discrimination in schematic eyes

To investigate whether the gaze discrimination impairment seen in Experiments 1 and 2 were gaze-specific, and not just a reflection of the residual spatial inattention per se, two types of schematic eyes (rectangular and elliptical) were designed and used as control tasks in direction discrimination.

Discussion

M.J., who after a near-complete damage of the right STG in her fifth, heretofore uneventful decade, became longstandingly hemianopic, but recovered gradually from her initial left hemispatial neglect with some subtle residual impairments that were no longer detectable by conventional neuropsychological assessment. The most puzzling of these impairments was a difficulty in obtaining eye contact. Taken together the implication that the STS (the upper bank of which comprises the STG) is involved

Conclusions

This study has demonstrated, in a case with a rare right STG damage, a novel deficit in discriminating contralesional gaze direction. We have suggested that the function of the STG in processing gaze, together with the directional information it derives from earlier visual areas like the MT/MST, may be associated, when damaged, with such a deficit. Concurrent with this deficit was an impaired ability to obtain eye-contact, implicating that the STG might also serve as an interface between the

References (46)

  • K.A. Pelphrey et al.

    Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: The influence of context

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • A. Senju et al.

    Eye contact does not facilitate detection in children with autism

    Cognition

    (2003)
  • M.J. Taylor et al.

    Direction of gaze effects on early face processing: Eyes-only versus full faces

    Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research

    (2001)
  • R. Walker et al.

    Disentangling neglect and hemianopia

    Neuropsychologia

    (1991)
  • B. Wicker et al.

    Brain regions involved in the perception of gaze: A PET study

    Neuroimage

    (1998)
  • S. Baron-Cohen

    Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind

    (1995)
  • J.J. Barton et al.

    Ocular search during line bisection. The effects of hemi-neglect and hemianopia

    Brain

    (1998)
  • J.J. Barton et al.

    Retinotopic and directional defects in motion discrimination in humans with cerebral lesions

    Annals of Neurology

    (1995)
  • J.J. Barton et al.

    Directional defects in pursuit and motion perception in humans with unilateral cerebral lesions

    Brain

    (1996)
  • E. Bonda et al.

    Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (1996)
  • R. Daini et al.

    Exploring the syndrome of spatial unilateral neglect through an illusion of length

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2002)
  • C. Friesen et al.

    The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze

    Psychonomic Bulletin and Review

    (1998)
  • E. Grossman et al.

    Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • Cited by (67)

    • Multimodal representations of person identity individuated with fMRI

      2017, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, monkeys with ablations of the right pSTS have been found to fail tasks that require perceiving the eye gaze direction of another face (Campbell, Heywood, Cowey, Regard, & Landis, 1990). Deficits for discriminating gaze direction were also observed in a patient with damage to the pSTG (Akiyama et al., 2006). Although it is difficult to assess whether the damaged regions include the human face-selective right pSTS, these results together with the imaging results point to an important role of the right pSTS also for the processing of dynamic aspects of faces, and the finding that this region encodes information about identity should not be taken to mean that it does not play a crucial role for the processing of dynamic faces.

    • Resting-state functional MRI of abnormal baseline brain activity in young depressed patients with and without suicidal behavior

      2016, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      The r-STG is a key cortical region in the processing of emotional information and cognitive regulation. It is associated with emotional attention (Narumoto et al., 2001), spatial perception and exploration (Ellison et al., 2004; Karnath et al., 2001) and facial processing (Morris et al., 1998), and lesions associated with difficulties in visual discrimination (Akiyama et al., 2006). The r-STG is activated during'Theory of Mind’ tasks (Vollm et al., 2006) suggesting that abnormal intrinsic brain activity in r-STG may represent a functional neural marker of social emotion in youths attempting suicide.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text