Neural bases of dysphoria in early Huntington's disease

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.04.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Psychiatric disorders, including disorders of emotion control, are common in Huntington's disease. The neurobiological mechanism of the increased rate of disorders of emotion control are not known. Emotion perception deficits have been reported in Huntington's disease, but studies of emotional experience have been limited. In the present study we aim to expand the research in emotion in Huntington's disease by examining the neural bases of induced dysphoria at an early stage of Huntington's disease. Ten Huntington's disease patients and 12 demographically matched healthy volunteers underwent [15O] water positron emission tomography while in a transient state of dysphoria induced by viewing negatively charged affect-laden stimuli. Both groups experienced dysphoric mood, but Huntington's disease patients responded to the stimuli with greater arousal, anger and fear than healthy controls. Induced dysphoric mood was associated with a widespread reduction of activity within the frontal and parietal lobes, thalamus, and cerebellum. These differences could not be explained based on the smaller gray matter volumes of the corresponding regions, although in Huntington's disease patients smaller caudate nucleus volumes predicted lower dorsal-lateral prefrontal activity. Areas of increased activity included the striate and extrastriate cortex, the left thalamus, the transverse temporal gyrus, and the posterior hippocampus. This study elucidates possible mechanisms contributing to psychiatric disturbances of emotion often found in patients with Huntington's disease.

Introduction

Huntington's disease is an autosomal dominant, neurodegenerative disorder that results from an unstable expansion of the trinucleotide repeat cytosine–adenine–guanine (CAG) of the gene IT-15 that codes for the protein huntingtin. Huntington's disease is relentless, leading to increasing functional disability and death over a period of 10–30 years (Paulsen et al., 2004). Clinical features of Huntington's disease usually arise in adulthood with the emergence of abnormal involuntary movements, psychiatric disturbance and cognitive impairment. Because functional decline often begins before overt clinical features, there has been a great emphasis on identifying preclinical manifestations of the disorder to allow for the possibility of early therapeutic interventions.

Psychiatric morbidity in Huntington's disease is multifaceted and severe, with uniformly high prevalence estimates of over 90% (Folstein et al., 1983, Paulsen et al., 2001). Psychiatric aspects of Huntington's disease constitute a major burden to patients and families, and are associated with more rapid functional deterioration (Marder et al., 2000), greater disability (Hamilton et al., 2003, Nehl and Paulsen, 2004), and earlier nursing home placement (Mayeux et al., 1986). Emotional disorders, psychosis, and personality changes with behavioral and emotional dyscontrol are most common.

Clinicians who care for patients with Huntington's disease often observe exaggerated responses to emotional stimuli. Family members often describe temper outbursts in situations where annoyance might have been expected. Similarly, persons with Huntington's disease describe feelings of sadness becoming severe depression and the experience of “nervousness” converting to incapacitating anxiety (Paulsen et al., 2001). Depression is a significant component of the overall psychiatric morbidity in Huntington's disease (Guttman et al., 2003), and is reported to be responsible for the premature end of life by suicide at a rate six times that in the general population (Schoenfeld et al., 1984, Almqvist et al., 1999, Paulsen et al., 2005). Emotional dyscontrol may manifest as personality changes and include the DSM-IV subtypes labile and disinhibited (Leroi et al., 2002).

Contributions to the development of psychopathology in Huntington's disease are multifactorial and likely include psychosocial factors (i.e., adjustment to a fatal genetic illness and increased disability), but the neuropathology of Huntington's disease suggests a preeminent role for biological mechanisms. Biological mechanisms may include changes in the brain regions subserving emotion processing. Deficits of recognition of facial disgust (Sprengelmeyer et al., 1996, Gray et al., 1997, Halligan, 1998, Milders et al., 2003, Wang et al., 2003, Hennenlotter et al., 2004) are consistent with the general notion of emotional disturbance of Huntington's disease patients. However, the neurobiological mechanism of the increased rate for the disorders of emotion control are not yet known.

Death of the GABAergic medium spiny neurons in the caudate nucleus is often observed in Huntington's disease, with additional involvement of the putamen, globus pallidus, and cerebral cortex (Vonsattel et al., 1985). Anatomical neuroimaging studies have shown volume loss up to 57% in the caudate, 64% in the putamen, and 21–25% in the amygdala in persons with mild Huntington's disease (Rosas et al., 2001). Mechanistically, abnormal emotional responses in Huntington's disease would be expected based on the dysfunction of the basal ganglia–thalamocortical circuitry (Alexander et al., 1990). Within this circuit, the ventral striatum (including the medial and ventral portions of the caudate nucleus and putamen, the nucleus accumbens and the striatal cells of the olfactory tubercle) receives projections from “limbic” structures, including the hippocampus, amygdala, entorhinal cortex and perirhinal cortex (Haber, 2003). Within this “limbic loop”, several prefrontal regions including the anterior cingulate and the medial orbitofrontal cortex exert regulatory functions through excitatory glutamatergic projections to the ventral striatum (Nakano et al., 2000). Then the ventral striatum sends inhibitory, GABA-mediated projections on to the ventral pallidum. The ventral pallidum continues the inhibitory output to the medial dorsal nucleus of the thalamus. The final connection in the loop is excitatory feedback from the thalamus, returning back to the anterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal cortex.

The motor (Alexander et al., 1990), cognitive (Aron et al., 2003), and emotional symptoms (Leroi et al., 2002) of Huntington's disease have been attributed to distant effects of basal ganglia damage to the thalamus and frontal lobes (Joel, 2001). Depending on the stage of the disease, frontal lobe dysfunction would directly or indirectly [via “release” of phylogenetic older structures—see Jackson's “theory of levels” (Jackson, 1876)] give way to “productive” or “positive” symptoms of Huntington's disease including chorea and ballism, hallucinations or mania, and emotional incontinence. The role of frontal lobe dysfunction in personality changes with labile mood and in depressive disorders is well established (Damasio et al., 2000, Rolls, 2004). In a parsimonious view, dysfunction of the frontal lobes would be the converging basis of the biological mechanisms for mood, personality and emotional psychopathology in Huntington's disease.

Whereas the frontal lobes are grossly affected later in the illness (Selemon et al., 2004), frontal lobe dysfunction is often observed in the early stages of the disease, even before motor symptom onset (Butters et al., 1978, Josiassen et al., 1982, Jason et al., 1988, Rothlind et al., 1993). Because functional and metabolic abnormalities are not consistently found in the early stages of the illness (Kuhl et al., 1982, Tanahashi et al., 1985, Young et al., 1986, Weinberger et al., 1988, Sax et al., 1996), subjects with Huntington's disease may need to be examined under specific mental conditions that elicit the frontal deficit.

Mood induction represents a valid method to probe neural structures subserving emotion in several neuropsychiatric disorders including depression, anxiety and schizophrenia (George et al., 1995, Morris et al., 1996, Lane et al., 1997, Paradiso et al., 2003a, Rauch et al., 2003). With the exception of research on perception of disgust in facial expressions (Sprengelmeyer et al., 1996, Gray et al., 1997, Halligan, 1998, Milders et al., 2003, Wang et al., 2003, Hennenlotter et al., 2004), studies on emotion processing in Huntington's disease have been limited. Because dysphoria has been found to be the most often endorsed psychiatric symptom in Huntington's disease (Paulsen et al., 2001), in the present study we aimed at expanding the research on the neural underpinnings of emotion processing in Huntington's disease by examining the functional neuroanatomy associated with an induced state of dysphoric mood in the early stages of the disease. We adopted a definition of dysphoria largely based on Andreasen and Black (2001). Dysphoria was defined as an altered state of emotion characterized by sadness, fear/anxiety, and/or tense irritability or anger.

Patients with established diagnosis of Huntington's disease (rather then pre-symptomatic gene carriers) were chosen for this study. Based on clinical observations and scientific reports (Paulsen et al., 2001), it was felt that this group of patients in the early stages of the disease would show the significant clinical phenomenon of exaggerated negative emotional response, including dysphoric mood (Paulsen et al., 2001). This study compared the brain activity elicited during a state of experimentally induced dysphoria recorded using [15O]water and positron emission tomography (PET) in patients in the early stages of Huntington's disease and in healthy comparison subjects controlling for the brain activity generated during a neutral mood state. Based on the role of the prefrontal cortex in emotion control (Rolls, 2004) and on prefrontal dysfunction in early Huntington's disease (Butters et al., 1978, Josiassen et al., 1982, Jason et al., 1988, Rothlind et al., 1993), we expected, at a minimum, that patients would show lower functional activity in frontal lobe structures compared with controls. Because of the expected group differences in brain structure (especially in the caudate nucleus and the frontal lobe), we carried out focused analyses aimed at determining the extent to which structural neuroanatomical changes would explain differences in emotional response and functional neuroanatomy.

Section snippets

Participants

Twelve persons with Huntington's disease were recruited from the Huntington's disease registry at the University of Iowa. The registry consists of persons diagnosed with Huntington's disease through clinical evaluation and confirmed via genetic testing as well as persons at risk for Huntington's disease (by virtue of family history) who have or have not undergone genetic testing. Participants with a polymerase chain reaction (PCR) confirmed CAG expansion in the IT-15 gene coding for huntingtin

Demographics and neurocognitive assessment

Demographic characteristics, anxiety and depression scores for participants who completed the study are shown in Table 1. Consistent with our clinical experience, patients with Huntington's disease showed greater anxiety and depression, but no subject met criteria for major depressive or generalized anxiety disorder. There was a statistically significant difference between the Huntington's disease and the control participants on the Hamilton Depression Scale. Two participants with Huntington's

Discussion

Dysphoria is a state of altered emotions characterized by fear, sadness, anger and mental discomfort. Dysphoric mood cuts across psychiatric diagnostic categories and is often observed in patients with Huntington's disease. In Huntington's disease, the behavioral counterpart of this altered mood state is often manifested by an elevated rate of temper outbursts in response to common daily events (Paulsen et al., 2001). The findings in this study offer the first neurobiological explanation of

Acknowledgments

Drs. Paradiso and Paulsen contributed equally to the conceptualization of this project. This study was supported by funds from the National Institutes of Health (NS40068, MH01579), the Roy J. Carver Charitable Trust and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute awarded to J.S. Paulsen, and from NIH MH00163 and MH52879 awarded to R.G. Robinson. Dr. Paradiso was supported by the Edward J. Mallinckrodt, Jr. Foundation and by an Institutional Mentored Career Development Award (K-12). Beth Turner was

References (103)

  • E.T. Rolls

    The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex

    Brain and Cognition

    (2004)
  • R. Spinks et al.

    Manual and automated measurement of the whole thalamus and mediodorsal nucleus using magnetic resonance imaging

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • B.M. Turner et al.

    The cerebellum and emotional experience

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • Y.D. Van der Werf et al.

    The intralaminar and midline nuclei of the thalamus. Anatomical and functional evidence for participation in processes of arousal and awareness

    Brain Research

    (2002)
  • K. Wang et al.

    Impairment of recognition of disgust in Chinese with Huntington's or Wilson's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • G.E. Alexander et al.

    Basal ganglia–thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, “prefrontal” and “limbic” functions

    Progress in Brain Research

    (1990)
  • American Psychiatric Association. Task Force on DSM-IV

    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV

    (1994)
  • A. Amos

    A computational model of information processing in the frontal cortex and basal ganglia

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • N.C. Andreasen et al.

    Introductory Textbook of Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • N.C. Andreasen et al.

    Image processing for the study of brain structure and function: problems and programs

    Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

    (1992)
  • N.C. Andreasen et al.

    Automatic atlas-based volume estimation of human brain regions from MR images

    Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography

    (1996)
  • S. Arndt et al.

    Tests for comparing images based on randomization and permutation methods

    Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism

    (1996)
  • A.R. Aron et al.

    Task-set switching deficits in early-stage Huntington's disease: implications for basal ganglia function

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • E.H. Aylward et al.

    Reduced basal ganglia volume associated with the gene for Huntington's disease in asymptomatic at-risk persons

    Neurology

    (1994)
  • E.H. Aylward et al.

    Frontal lobe volume in patients with Huntington's disease

    Neurology

    (1998)
  • P. Beck et al.

    The Hamilton Scales

    (1990)
  • L.J. Beglinger et al.

    White matter volume and cognitive dysfunction in early Huntington's disease

    Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology

    (2005)
  • A.L. Benton

    Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Clinical Manual

    (1994)
  • R.J. Blair

    Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • Blair, R.J., Cipolotti, L., 2000. Impaired social response reversal. A case of ‘acquired sociopathy’. Brain 123 (Pt 6),...
  • M.C. Brower et al.

    Neuropsychiatry of frontal lobe dysfunction in violent and criminal behaviour: a critical review

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • C.M. Butter et al.

    Alterations in aversive and aggressive behaviors following orbital frontal lesions in rhesus monkeys

    Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis (Wars)

    (1972)
  • N. Butters et al.

    Comparison of the neuropsychological deficits associated with early and advanced Huntington's disease

    Archives of Neurology

    (1978)
  • P.W. Corson et al.

    Caudate size in first-episode neuroleptic–naive schizophrenic patients measured using an artificial neural network

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • A.R. Damasio et al.

    Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • S. Folstein et al.

    The association of affective disorder with Huntington's disease in a case series and in families

    Psychological Medicine

    (1983)
  • M.S. George et al.

    Brain regions involved in recognizing facial emotion or identity: an oxygen-15 PET study

    Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

    (1993)
  • M.S. George et al.

    Brain activity during transient sadness and happiness in healthy women

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (1995)
  • E. Gomez-Tortosa et al.

    Relation between frontal lobe blood flow and cognitive performance in Huntington's disease

    Neurologia

    (1996)
  • J.M. Gray et al.

    Impaired recognition of disgust in Huntington's disease gene carriers

    Brain

    (1997)
  • M. Guttman et al.

    Clinical management of aggression and frontal symptoms in Huntington's disease

  • M. Hamilton

    Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness

    British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology

    (1967)
  • M. Hamilton

    Diagnosis and rating of anxiety

    British Journal of Psychiatry, Special Publication

    (1969)
  • J.M. Hamilton et al.

    Behavioural abnormalities contribute to functional decline in Huntington's disease

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • A.R. Hariri et al.

    Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system

    NeuroReport

    (2000)
  • R.K. Heaton et al.

    Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual: Revised and Expanded

    (1981)
  • A. Hennenlotter et al.

    Neural correlates associated with impaired disgust processing in pre-symptomatic Huntington's disease

    Brain

    (2004)
  • B. Horwitz

    Data analysis paradigms for metabolic-flow data: combining neural modeling and functional neuroimaging

    Human Brain Mapping

    (1994)
  • Huntington's Study Group

    Unified Huntington's Disease Rating Scale: reliability and consistency. Huntington Study Group

    Movement Disorders

    (1996)
  • H. Jackson

    West Riding Asylum Reports

    (1876)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text