Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T23:31:52.542Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Semantic memory is both impaired and anomalous in schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

E. Y. H. Chen
Affiliation:
Fulbourn Hospital, MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge
A. J. Wilkins
Affiliation:
Fulbourn Hospital, MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge
P. J. McKenna*
Affiliation:
Fulbourn Hospital, MRC Applied Psychology Unit, Cambridge
*
1Address for correspondence: Dr P. J. McKenna, Fulbourn Hospital, Cambridge CB1 5EF.

Synopsis

The integrity of semantic memory in schizophrenia was examined in a reaction time task requiring subjects to verify words as members or non-members of a conceptual category, where the words differed in their degree of semantic relationship to the category. Compared to matched normal controls, 28 schizophrenic patients were impaired on the task, showing slower responses in all conditions. In addition, their performance was anomalous in that they took longest to respond to items that were outside the category but semantically related to it, in contrast to the controls who took the longest to respond to ambiguous words at the borderline of the category. The pattern of ‘yes’ and ‘no’ responses of the patients was anomalous in a similar way. In both speed and accuracy of responding, the findings indicate that there is an outward shift of semantic category boundaries in schizophrenia.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, J. A. (1989). Categorization and selective neurons. In Parallel Models of Associative Memory (ed. Hinton, G. E. and Anderson, J. A.), pp. 251274. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1987). The Comprehensive Assessment of Symptoms and History. University of Iowa College of Medicine: Iowa City.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human Memory: Theory and Practice. Erlbaum: Hove.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Harris, J., Sunderland, A., Watts, K. P. & Wilson, B. A. (1987). Closed head injury and memory. In Neurobehavioural Recovery from Head Injury (ed. Levin, H. S., Grafman, J. and Eisenberg, H. M.), pp. 295317. Oxford University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Lewis, V., Eldridge, M. & Thomson, N. (1984). Attention and retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113, 518540.Google Scholar
Battig, W. F. & Montague, W. E. (1969). Category norms for verbal items in five categories: a replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms. Journal of Experimental Psychology (Monograph) 80, 145.Google Scholar
Braff, D. L., Heaton, R., Kuck, J., Cullum, M., Moranville, J., Grant, I. & Zisook, S. (1991). The generalized pattern of neuropsychological deficits in outpatients with chronic schizophrenia with heterogeneous Wisconsin Card Sorting Test results. Archives of General Psychiatry 48, 891898.Google Scholar
Cameron, N. (1939). Schizophrenic thinking in a problem-solving situation. Journal of Mental Science 85, 10121035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, N. (1954). Experimental analysis of schizophrenic thinking. In Language and Thought in Schizophrenia (ed. Kasanin, J. S.), pp. 5063. University of California Press: Berkeley and Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Chang, T. M. (1986). Semantic memory: facts and models. Psychological Bulletin 99, 199220.Google Scholar
Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J. P. (1973). Disordered Thought in Schizophrenia. Appleton–Century–Crofts: New York.Google Scholar
Collins, A. M. & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review 82, 407428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, A. M. & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 8, 240247.Google Scholar
Cutting, J. (1985). The Psychology of Schizophrenia. Churchill Livingstone: Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Cutting, J. & Murphy, D. (1988). Schizophrenic thought disorder: a psychological and organic interpretation. British Journal of Psychiatry 152, 310319.Google Scholar
Endicott, J., Spitzer, R. C., Fliess, J. L. & Cohen, J. (1976). The global assessment scale. Archives of General Psychiatry 33,766771.Google Scholar
George, L. & Neufeld, R. W. J. (1985). Cognition and symptomatology in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 11, 264285.Google Scholar
Hampton, J. A. & Gardiner, M. M. (1983). Measures of internal category structure: correlational analysis of normative data. British Journal of Psychology 74, 491516.Google Scholar
Hawks, D. V. & Payne, R. W. (1971). Overinclusive thought disorder and symptomatology. British Journal of Psychiatry 118, 663670.Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E. (1989). Implementing semantic networks in parallel hardware. In Parallel Models of Associative Memory (ed. Hinton, G. E. and Anderson, J. A.), pp. 191217. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1980). Semantic memory: a tutorial. In Attention and Performance, VIII (ed. Nickerson, R. S.), pp. 595620. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
Koh, S. D. (1978). Remembering of verbal materials by schizophrenic young adults. In Language and Cognition in Schizophrenia (ed. Schwartz, S.), pp. 5599. Erlbaum: Hillsdale, New Jersey.Google Scholar
Kolb, B. & Whishaw, I. Q. (1983). Performance of schizophrenic patients on tests sensitive to right or left frontal, temporal or parietal function in neurological patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 171, 435443.Google Scholar
Kwapil, T. R., Hegley, D. C., Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J. P. (1990). Facilitation of word recognition by semantic priming in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 99, 215221.Google Scholar
Lund, C. E., Mortimer, A. M., McKenna, P. J. & Rogers, D. (1991). Motor, volitional and behavioural disorders in schizophrenia. 1. Assessment using the Modified Rogers Scale. British Journal of Psychiatry 158, 323327.Google Scholar
McCloskey, M. (1980). The stimulus familiarity problem in semantic memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 19, 485502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKenna, P. J., Tamlyn, D., Lund, C. E., Mortimer, A. M., Hammond, S. & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Amnesic syndrome in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 20, 967972.Google Scholar
Manschreck, T. C., Maher, B. A., Milavetz, J. J., Ames, D., Weisstein, C. C. & Schneyer, M. L. (1988). Semantic priming in thought disordered schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research 1, 6166.Google Scholar
Metzger, R. L., Miller, M. L., Cohen, M., Sofka, M. & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Worry changes decision making: the effect of negative thoughts on cognitive processing. Journal of Clinical Psychology 46, 7888.Google Scholar
Montgomery, S. A. & Åsberg, M. (1979). A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. British Journal of Psychiatry 139, 382389.Google Scholar
Mortimer, A. M., Lund, C. E., McKenna, P. J. & Mannuzza, S. (1989). Rating of negative symptoms using the HEN scale. British Journal of Psychiatry 155, suppl. 7, 8992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, H. E. (1982). The National Adult Reading Test (NART). NFER-Nelson: Windsor, Berks.Google Scholar
Owens, D. G. C. & Johnstone, E. C. (1990). The disabilities of chronic schizophrenia: their nature and the factors contributing to their development. British Journal of Psychiatry 136, 384393.Google Scholar
Payne, R. W. (1973). Cognitive abnormalities. In Handbook of Abnormal Psychology (ed. Eysenck, H. J.), pp. 420483. Pitman: London.Google Scholar
Saykin, A. J., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., Mozley, P. D., Mozley, L. H., Resnick, S. M., Kester, D. B. & Stafiniak, P. (1991). Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia: selective impairment in memory and learning. Archives of General Psychiatry 48, 618624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, G. M. & Angus, J. W. S. (1970). Drug induced extrapyramidal disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, suppl. 212, 158.Google Scholar
Simpson, G. M., Lee, J. H., Zoubok, B. & Gardos, G. (1979). A rating scale for tardive dyskinesia. Psychopharmacology 64, 171179.Google Scholar
Smith, E. E., Shoben, E. J. & Rips, L. J. (1974). Structure and process in semantic memory: a featural model for semantic decisions. Psychological Review 81, 214241.Google Scholar
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J. & Robins, E. (1978). Research Diagnostic Criteria for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders. Biometric Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York.Google Scholar
Tamlyn, D., McKenna, P. J., Mortimer, A. M., Lund, C. E., Hammond, S. & Badeley, A. D. (1992). Memory impairment in schizophrenia: its extent, affiliations and neuropsychological character. Psychological Medicine 22, 101115.Google Scholar
Toothaker, L. (1991). Multiple Comparisons for Researchers. Sage Publications: California.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In Organization of Memory (ed. Tulving, E. and Donaldson, W.), pp. 382404. Academic Press: New York.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford University Press: Oxford.Google Scholar
Wilkins, A. J. (1971). Conjoint frequency, category size, and categorization time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 10, 382385.Google Scholar
Yates, A. J. (1973). Abnormalities of psychomotor functions. In Handbook of Abnormal Psychology (ed. Eysenck, H. J.), pp. 261283. Pitman: London.Google Scholar