Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-fqc5m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T02:56:48.371Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Access of emotional information to visual awareness in patients with major depressive disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2011

P. Sterzer*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité – UniversitätsmedizinBerlin, Germany
T. Hilgenfeldt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité – UniversitätsmedizinBerlin, Germany
P. Freudenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité – UniversitätsmedizinBerlin, Germany
F. Bermpohl
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité – UniversitätsmedizinBerlin, Germany
M. Adli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charité – UniversitätsmedizinBerlin, Germany
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr P. Sterzer, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Campus Charité Mitte, Charitéplatz 1, D-10117 Berlin, Germany. (Email: philipp.sterzer@charite.de)

Abstract

Background

According to cognitive theories of depression, negative biases affect most cognitive processes including perception. Such depressive perception may result not only from biased cognitive appraisal but also from automatic processing biases that influence the access of sensory information to awareness.

Method

Twenty patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) and 20 healthy control participants underwent behavioural testing with a variant of binocular rivalry, continuous flash suppression (CFS), to investigate the potency of emotional visual stimuli to gain access to awareness. While a neutral, fearful, happy or sad emotional face was presented to one eye, high-contrast dynamic patterns were presented to the other eye, resulting in initial suppression of the face from awareness. Participants indicated the location of the face with a key press as soon as it became visible. The modulation of suppression time by emotional expression was taken as an index of unconscious emotion processing.

Results

We found a significant difference in the emotional modulation of suppression time between MDD patients and controls. This difference was due to relatively shorter suppression of sad faces and, to a lesser degree, to longer suppression of happy faces in MDD. Suppression time modulation by sad expression correlated with change in self-reported severity of depression after 4 weeks.

Conclusions

Our finding of preferential access to awareness for mood-congruent stimuli supports the notion that depressive perception may be related to altered sensory information processing even at automatic processing stages. Such perceptual biases towards mood-congruent information may reinforce depressed mood and contribute to negative cognitive biases.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alpers, GW, Gerdes, AB (2007). Here is looking at you: emotional faces predominate in binocular rivalry. Emotion 7, 495506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amting, JM, Greening, SG, Mitchell, DG (2010). Multiple mechanisms of consciousness: the neural correlates of emotional awareness. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 1003910047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arce, E, Simmons, AN, Lovero, KL, Stein, MB, Paulus, MP (2008). Escitalopram effects on insula and amygdala BOLD activation during emotional processing. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 196, 661672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A (1976). Cognitive Therapy and Emotional Disorders. International Universities Press: Madison, CT.Google Scholar
Beck, A, Rush, AJ, Shaw, B, Emery, G (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression. Guilford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Beck, AT (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 969977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blake, R, Logothetis, NK (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3, 1321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, BP, Mogg, K, Millar, N (1996). Implicit memory bias in clinical and non-clinical depression. Behavior Research and Therapy 34, 865879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, BP, Mogg, K, Williams, R (1995). Implicit and explicit memory for emotion-congruent information in clinical depression and anxiety. Behavior Research and Therapy 33, 755770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browning, M, Reid, C, Cowen, PJ, Goodwin, GM, Harmer, CJ (2007). A single dose of citalopram increases fear recognition in healthy subjects. Journal of Psychopharmacology 21, 684690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dearing, KF, Gotlib, IH (2009). Interpretation of ambiguous information in girls at risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 37, 7991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feinberg, TE, Rifkin, A, Schaffer, C, Walker, E (1986). Facial discrimination and emotional recognition in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 43, 276279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilboa-Schechtman, E, Ben-Artzi, E, Jeczemien, P, Marom, S, Hermesh, H (2004). Depression impairs the ability to ignore the emotional aspects of facial expressions: evidence from the Garner task. Cognition and Emotion 18, 209231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, IH, Kasch, KL, Traill, S, Joormann, J, Arnow, BA, Johnson, SL (2004 a). Coherence and specificity of information-processing biases in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 113, 386398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, IH, Krasnoperova, E, Yue, DN, Joormann, J (2004 b). Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 113, 121135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, KL, Adams, WJ, Garner, M (2009). The influence of anxiety on the initial selection of emotional faces presented in binocular rivalry. Cognition 113, 105110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, RC, Erwin, RJ, Gur, RE, Zwil, AS, Heimberg, C, Kraemer, HC (1992). Facial emotion discrimination: II. Behavioral findings in depression. Psychiatry Research 42, 241251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, WW 3rd (1998). Judgment of facial expressions and depression persistence. Psychiatry Research 80, 265274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, WW 3rd, Jansen, JH, Bouhuys, AL, van den Hoofdakker, RH (1998). The judgement of facial expressions by depressed patients, their partners and controls. Journal of Affective Disorders 47, 6370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, CJ, Hill, SA, Taylor, MJ, Cowen, PJ, Goodwin, GM (2003). Toward a neuropsychological theory of antidepressant drug action: increase in positive emotional bias after potentiation of norepinephrine activity. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 990992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmer, CJ, Mackay, CE, Reid, CB, Cowen, PJ, Goodwin, GM (2006). Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues. Biological Psychiatry 59, 816820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ihssen, N, Heim, S, Keil, A (2007). The costs of emotional attention: affective processing inhibits subsequent lexico-semantic analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19, 19321949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiang, Y, Costello, P, He, S (2007). Processing of invisible stimuli: advantage of upright faces and recognizable words in overcoming interocular suppression. Psychological Science 18, 349355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joormann, J (2004). Attentional biases in dysphoria: the role of inhibitory processes. Cognition and Emotion 18, 125147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joormann, J, Gotlib, IH (2006). Is this happiness I see? Biases in the identification of emotional facial expressions in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115, 705714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joormann, J, Talbot, L, Gotlib, IH (2007). Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 116, 135143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeMoult, J, Joormann, J, Sherdell, L, Wright, Y, Gotlib, IH (2009). Identification of emotional facial expressions following recovery from depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 118, 828833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leopold, DA, Logothetis, NK (1999). Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception. Trends in Cognitive Science 3, 254264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppanen, JM (2006). Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 19, 3439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppanen, JM, Milders, M, Bell, JS, Terriere, E, Hietanen, JK (2004). Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces. Psychiatry Research 128, 123133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meng, M, Tong, F (2004). Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. Journal of Vision 4, 539551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, SM, Gynther, BD, Heslop, KR, Liu, GB, Mitchell, PB, Ngo, TT, Pettigrew, JD, Geffen, LB (2003). Slow binocular rivalry in bipolar disorder. Psychological Medicine 33, 683692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagamine, M, Yoshino, A, Miyazaki, M, Takahashi, Y, Nomura, S (2009). Difference in binocular rivalry rate between patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorders. Bipolar Disorder 11, 539546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Persad, SM, Polivy, J (1993). Differences between depressed and nondepressed individuals in the recognition of and response to facial emotional cues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 102, 358368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubinow, DR, Post, RM (1992). Impaired recognition of affect in facial expression in depressed patients. Biological Psychiatry 31, 947953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterzer, P, Frith, C, Petrovic, P (2008 a). Believing is seeing: expectations alter visual awareness. Current Biology 18, R697R698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterzer, P, Haynes, JD, Rees, G (2008 b). Fine-scale activity patterns in high-level visual areas encode the category of invisible objects. Journal of Vision 8, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterzer, P, Jalkanen, L, Rees, G (2009 a). Electromagnetic responses to invisible face stimuli during binocular suppression. NeuroImage 46, 803808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterzer, P, Kleinschmidt, A, Rees, G (2009 b). The neural bases of multistable perception. Trends in Cognitive Science 13, 310318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surguladze, S, Brammer, MJ, Keedwell, P, Giampietro, V, Young, AW, Travis, MJ, Williams, SC, Phillips, ML (2005). A differential pattern of neural response toward sad versus happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 57, 201209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suslow, T, Dannlowski, U, Lalee-Mentzel, J, Donges, US, Arolt, V, Kersting, A (2004). Spatial processing of facial emotion in patients with unipolar depression: a longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders 83, 5963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suslow, T, Junghanns, K, Arolt, V (2001). Detection of facial expressions of emotions in depression. Perception and Motor Skills 92, 857868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suslow, T, Konrad, C, Kugel, H, Rumstadt, D, Zwitserlood, P, Schoning, S, Ohrmann, P, Bauer, J, Pyka, M, Kersting, A, Arolt, V, Heindel, W, Dannlowski, U (2010). Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in major depression. Biological Psychiatry 67, 155160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tong, F, Meng, M, Blake, R (2006). Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends in Cognitive Science 10, 502511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsuchiya, N, Koch, C (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience 8, 10961101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsuchiya, N, Moradi, F, Felsen, C, Yamazaki, M, Adolphs, R (2009). Intact rapid detection of fearful faces in the absence of the amygdala. Nature Neuroscience 12, 12241225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, L, LaBar, KS, McCarthy, G (2006). Mood alters amygdala activation to sad distractors during an attentional task. Biological Psychiatry 60, 11391146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, E, Zald, DH, Blake, R (2007). Fearful expressions gain preferential access to awareness during continuous flash suppression. Emotion 7, 882886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoon, KL, Hong, SW, Joormann, J, Kang, P (2009). Perception of facial expressions of emotion during binocular rivalry. Emotion 9, 172182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, W, Jiang, Y, He, S, Chen, D (2010). Olfaction modulates visual perception in binocular rivalry. Current Biology 20, 13561358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed