Skip to main content
Log in

Are Deficits in the Decoding of Affective Cues and in Mentalizing Abilities Independent?

  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It has been hypothesized that deficits in theory of mind (ToM) and emotion recognition abilities in subjects with autisticlike disorders are independent. We examined the relationships between deficits in the various social cognitive domains in children with an autistic disorder (N = 20), in children with a pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDDNOS) (N = 20), and in psychiatric control (N = 20) and normal children (N = 20). The clinical groups were matched person-to-person on age and verbal IQ. The clinical children were 8–18 years old, the normal children 8–13 years old. The test battery included tasks for the matching and the context recognition of emotional expressions, and a set of first- and second-order ToM tasks. ToM and emotion recognition functioning proved to be better integrated in the non-PDD children than in the PDD children, but also in the PDD children significant correlations were found between ToM and emotion recognition measures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Achenbach, T. M, & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W., Harris, P. L., & Olson, D. R. (1988). Developing theories of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bachevalier, J. (1994). Medial temporal lobe structures and autism: Review of clinical and experimental findings. Neuropsychologia, 32, 627–648.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1988). Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: cognitive or affective? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 379–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989a). Are autistic children “behaviorists”? An examination of their mental-physical and appearance reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 579–600.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989b). The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology: The development of a theory of mind, and its dysfunction. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds. Perspectives from autism (pp. 59–82). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind?” Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioral and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Moriarty, J., Schmitz, B., Costa, D., & Ell, P. (1994). Recognition of mental state terms—Clinical findings in children with autism and a functional neuroimaging study of normal adults. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 640–649.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benes, F. M. (1994). Development of the corticolimbic system. In G. Dawson & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing mind (pp. 176–206). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brothers, L., & Ring, B. (1992). A neuroethological framework for the representation of minds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 107–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Friezen, W. U., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face. New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fein, D., Lucci, D., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse, L. (1992). Comprehension of affect in context in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1157–1167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford, U.K.: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1994a). Wechsler IQ profile and theory of mind in autism: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1461–1471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1994b). Annotation: Current psychological theories of autism: the “theory of mind” account and rival theories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 215–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L. (1994). The child's understanding of emotion: Developmental change and the family environment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 3–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1986a). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion: a further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 671–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1986b). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 321–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1989). Beyond cognition. In G. Dawson (Ed.), Autism, Nature, Diagnosis and Treatment (pp. 22–48). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • lannotti, R. J. (1985). Naturalistic and structured assessments of prosocial behavior in preschool children: The influence of empathy and perspective taking. Developmental Psychology, 21, 46–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1972). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, R. (1992). The limbic system. Emotion, laterality, and unconscious mind. In R. Joseph (Ed.), Neuropsychology, neuropsychiatry, and behavioral neurology (pp. 87–xxx). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1990). Prospects for a cognitive neuropsychology of autism: Hobson's choice. Psychological Review, 97, 122–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989a). The theoretical implications of joint attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989b). Second thoughts on the nature of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 213–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B., & Rogers, S. (1990). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic children: relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 31, 1081–1105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekam, S. R. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: knowledge, belief and communication. Child Development, 60, 689–700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that ...” Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5–10 years old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M., Dahlstrom, B., & Squires, T.-L. (1990). Autistic children's knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 587–601.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., DeVellis, R. F., & Daly, K. (1980). Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10, 91–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slomkowski, C, & Dunn, J. (1996). Young children's understanding of other people's beliefs and feelings and their connected communication with friends. Developmental Psychology, 32, 442–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, N. L, & Trabasso, T. (1989). Children's understanding of changing emotional states. In G Saarni & P. L. Harris (Eds.), Children's understanding of emotion (pp. 50–77). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strayer, J. (1980). A naturalistic study of empathic behaviors and their relation to affective states and perspective-taking skills in preschool children. Child Development, 51, 815–822.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strayer, J. (1987). Affective and cognitive perspectives on empathy. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 218–244). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, D. M. (1992). Developing emotions and cortical networks. In M. R. Gunnar & C. A. Nelson (Eds.), Developmental behavioral neuroscience (pp. 75–128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126–136.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Buitelaar, J.K., van der Wees, M. Are Deficits in the Decoding of Affective Cues and in Mentalizing Abilities Independent?. J Autism Dev Disord 27, 539–556 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025878026569

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025878026569

Keywords

Navigation