The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticlesFull Access

No Association of Lower Hippocampal Volume With Alzheimer’s Disease Pathology in Late-Life Depression

Abstract

Objective:

Hippocampal volume is commonly decreased in late-life depression. According to the depression-as-late-life-neuropsychiatric-disorder model, lower hippocampal volume in late-life depression is associated with neurodegenerative changes. The purpose of this prospective study was to examine whether lower hippocampal volume in late-life depression is associated with Alzheimer’s disease pathology.

Method:

Of 108 subjects who participated, complete, good-quality data sets were available for 100: 48 currently depressed older adults and 52 age- and gender-matched healthy comparison subjects who underwent structural MRI, [18F]flutemetamol amyloid positron emission tomography imaging, apolipoprotein E genotyping, and neuropsychological assessment. Hippocampal volumes were defined manually and normalized for total intracranial volume. Amyloid binding was quantified using the standardized uptake value ratio in one cortical composite volume of interest. The authors investigated group differences in hippocampal volume (both including and excluding amyloid-positive participants), group differences in amyloid uptake and in the proportion of positive amyloid scans, and the association between hippocampal volume and cortical amyloid uptake.

Results:

A significant difference was observed in mean normalized total hippocampal volume between patients and comparison subjects, but there were no group differences in cortical amyloid uptake or proportion of amyloid-positive subjects. The difference in hippocampal volume remained significant after the amyloid-positive subjects were excluded. There was no association between hippocampal volume and amyloid uptake in either patients or comparison subjects.

Conclusions:

Lower hippocampal volume was not related to amyloid pathology in this sample of patients with late-life depression. These data counter the common belief that changes in hippocampal volume in late-life depression are due to prodromal Alzheimer’s disease.

With some exceptions (1, 2), there is accumulating evidence showing that late-life depression is associated with smaller hippocampal volume (39). However, there is considerable debate about its etiology. The leading hypotheses are the depression-as-late-life-neuropsychiatric-disorder model (7, 9, 10) and the stress model (11).

The neuropsychiatric model proposes that hippocampal volume loss in late-life depression is caused by age-related neurodegenerative or other pathological brain changes (9). It has been repeatedly shown that in a significant proportion of patients, late-life depression precedes the onset of dementia, particularly Alzheimer’s disease (12), which is itself associated with hippocampal atrophy. Correspondingly, hippocampal volume decrease has been associated with later dementia (10) and continuing memory deficits during follow-up (4). Most studies document greater volume loss in late-onset compared with early-onset depression, and specific volume changes in hippocampal subfields involved in Alzheimer’s disease have been demonstrated in late-onset depression (5). Neuropathological studies show Alzheimer-type pathology in the majority of patients with a diagnosis of late-life depression who subsequently develop dementia (13), as well as a disproportionate amount of neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in the hippocampus of patients with Alzheimer’s disease and a history of depression (14). However, a neuropathological study in late-life depression without dementia (15) documented hippocampal neuronal loss in the absence of Alzheimer-type or cerebrovascular pathology, suggesting an alternative cause of hippocampal volume loss. Furthermore, in a clinical-pathological cohort study (16), depressive symptoms were associated with cognitive decline independently of neuropathological hallmarks of dementia.

The main alternative to the neuropsychiatric model is the stress hypothesis, which posits that stress-related mechanisms, including toxicity by elevated cortisol levels, reduced neurotrophic factors, and excess glutamatergic neurotransmission, have detrimental effects on the hippocampus, causing volume decrease (17). Support for this hypothesis comes from studies linking a longer duration of depression to smaller hippocampal volumes (18, 19) and from population-based studies showing that depression precedes hippocampal volume decrease (20). Accordingly, one would expect that chronic stress throughout the lifespan would have a cumulative pathological effect on the hippocampus, causing disproportionate volume decrease in the elderly, especially those with an early-onset depression. Although some reports support this prediction (6, 21), the majority find more hippocampal atrophy in late-onset depression (3, 5, 7) or fail to find an effect of illness onset (8). On the other hand, there is increasing evidence that early life stress in genetically predisposed individuals may contribute to hippocampal volume loss before illness onset (17) and that lower hippocampal volume increases vulnerability to psychological trauma and depression (22, 23).

The stress model and the depression-as-late-life-neuropsychiatric-disorder model are not mutually exclusive and may act synergistically. It has repeatedly been shown that depression increases the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease later in life (24). Studies in animal models of Alzheimer’s disease show that chronic stress accelerates amyloid deposition in mice (25). There may also be a common genetic predisposition for the adverse effects of stress and the development of Alzheimer’s disease. For instance, gene-environment interaction studies indicate that apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 carriers are more vulnerable to the adverse effects of prolonged stress on cognitive aging (26).

Thus far, support for the depression-as-late-life-neuropsychiatric-disorder model comes mainly from longitudinal studies in which often considerable time elapsed between the depression and incident dementia or neuropathological examination. Hence, based on the available evidence, it remains difficult to determine whether hippocampal atrophy in late-life depression represents an early sign of Alzheimer’s disease or is due to another cause. With the advent of positron emission tomography (PET) amyloid imaging, it has become possible to detect preclinical Alzheimer’s disease pathology in vivo (27, 28). Amyloid PET imaging has been used to study whether a history of depression in the elderly (29, 30) or varying degrees of cognitive impairment in late-life depression (31) are associated with amyloid pathology, yielding mixed results. However, the relationship between hippocampal volume and amyloid load has not yet been addressed in individuals who are depressed at the time of assessment. In this study, we investigated prospectively whether hippocampal volume in elderly patients with a current diagnosis of major depressive disorder was related to brain amyloid levels as measured by [18F]flutemetamol PET.

Method

Participants

Patients with late-life depression.

A consecutive series of 55 patients with late-life depression was recruited from the Department of Old Age Psychiatry of the University Psychiatric Center, KU Leuven, Belgium. Ultimately, 51 of these patients participated in the study (two withdrew, one was excluded because of a large intracerebral vascular lesion, and one was excluded because of a technical difficulty during PET scanning). To be included, patients had to be over age 60, have major depressive disorder according to DSM-IV criteria, and not have been primarily referred for assessment of cognitive impairment. Exclusion criteria were comorbid major psychiatric illness, previous or current alcohol or drug dependence, neurological illness (including stroke, transient ischemic attacks, and dementia), illness or medication precluding cognitive testing, metal implants precluding MRI scanning, and ECT in the past 6 months.

Healthy comparison subjects.

We included 53 age- and gender-matched comparison subjects who had no current or prior depressive illness or cognitive impairment, as determined by formal clinical and neuropsychological assessment (details below). Exclusion criteria were the same as for the patient group. Part of this cohort (N=46) was recruited from a larger [18F]flutemetamol study conducted at our center (32). This subset fulfilled additional inclusion criteria: they had a Clinical Dementia Rating score of 0 and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score >26, and they were stratified for APOE ε4 allele. The APOE ε4 status of the stratified comparison subjects did not differ from those of the patient group or the nonstratified comparison group. Recruitment was done through an existing departmental database and advertisement in a local newspaper.

All participants were genotyped to determine APOE status and provided written informed consent prior to enrollment in the study in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the ethical committee of the University Hospitals Leuven (protocol S52151) and registered as a clinical trial (EudraCT Number 2009-018064-95).

Assessment of Mood and Cognition

The diagnostic status of patients and comparison subjects was assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. We used the Geriatric Depression Scale to score depression severity and the MMSE for global cognition. An extensive neuropsychological examination including the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test was used to study the association between episodic memory and the imaging measures. Other neuropsychological tests are listed in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article.

Structural MRI

Acquisition.

High-resolution T1-weighted images were acquired on a 3-T Philips Intera scanner (Best, the Netherlands) with an eight-channel head coil using a three-dimensional turbo field echo sequence (TR=9.6 seconds, TE=4.6 seconds, flip angle=8°, in-plane voxel size=0.98×0.98×1.2 mm3, 182 slices). Because the scanner was decommissioned during the study, 19 of the comparison subjects underwent scanning with identical sequence parameters on a 3-T Philips Achieva scanner.

Hippocampal volume measurement.

Hippocampal volumes were defined manually in native space after an initial automatic segmentation step based on that described by Jain et al. (33), using training labels based on the Harmonized Protocol (34). Manual editing was performed by a single trained rater blind to participant diagnosis, using ITK-SNAP (http://www.itksnap.org/pmwiki/pmwiki.php) in accordance with the Harmonized Protocol guidelines (34) (Figure 1). Hippocampal volumes were normalized using the linear regression coefficient obtained by regressing total brain volume with gray matter volume, entered into the following equation: normalized hippocampal volume = original hippocampal volume − coefficient × (total intracranial volume − mean total intracranial volume) (34). Total intracranial volume was obtained from an automated segmentation of gray matter, white matter, and CSF (35). Intrarater reliability was determined using randomly selected scans delineated twice, at least 1 month apart. The intraclass correlation coefficient (Cronbach’s alpha) was 0.96 for the left hippocampus and 0.95 for the right hippocampus.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Imaging Techniques Used in the Analysisa

a The left-hand panel presents examples of axial standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) maps of an amyloid-positive participant, an amyloid-negative participant, and the volumes of interest (left and right lateral frontal, temporal, and parietal cortex and anterior and posterior cingulate cortex) used to create the composite SUVR (VOIcomp) overlaid on the amyloid-negative participant (desaturated for visualization purposes). The right-hand panel presents an example of a sagittal axial T1 anatomical scan with the hippocampus manually delineated in red (top image) and a three-dimensional rendering of the left hippocampus (bottom image).

White matter hyperintensities.

Given the association between late-life depression and white matter disease, we also quantified total white matter hyperintensity volume using a validated automatic segmentation approach (35). T1 and fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) data were combined in a probabilistic model in which white matter hyperintense lesions are detected as outliers when segmenting brain tissue into gray matter, white matter, and CSF compartments. White matter hyperintensity volume was normalized for total intracranial volume.

Amyloid Imaging

Acquisition.

[18F]Flutemetamol PET imaging was performed at the University Hospitals Leuven, as described previously (28, 36). Images were acquired on a 16-slice Siemens Biograph PET/CT scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). The tracer was injected intravenously as a bolus (mean activity, 149.5 MBq, SD=4.7, range=139–159) in an antecubital vein. Image acquisition started 90 minutes after tracer injection and lasted for 30 minutes in list mode. Prior to the PET scan, a low-dose CT scan was performed for attenuation correction. Random and scatter corrections were also applied. Images were reconstructed to six frames of 5 minutes, using an ordered subsets expectation maximization algorithm (4 iterations × 16 subsets).

Processing.

All reconstructed images were analyzed using SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London) in the MATLAB environment (MathWorks, Natick, Mass.). All six PET frames were realigned to the first frame to correct for potential head motion. Subsequently, the realigned frames were summed to create one PET summed image. Individual T1-weighted MRI images were coregistered to this PET summed image. These MRI images were then normalized to Montreal Neurological Institute space, after which the normalization matrix was applied to the coregistered PET summed images. Standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) images were calculated from the normalized PET summed image using cerebellar gray matter as reference region. The cerebellar gray matter volume of interest was derived from the Automated Anatomical Labeling Atlas (AAL) (36) and was masked with the normalized subject-specific segmented gray matter map to exclude white matter content.

The mean SUVR value was calculated in five bilateral volumes of interest derived from the AAL atlas: lateral frontal, parietal, and temporal cortex and anterior and posterior cingulate cortex (Figure 1). A composite cortical SUVR value (SUVRcomp) was calculated by averaging across these five volumes of interest. As with the cerebellar gray matter volume of interest, cortical volumes of interest were masked by the normalized subject-specific gray matter map (36). A SUVRcomp threshold of 1.38 was used to discriminate between a positive and a negative amyloid scan, as this threshold was recently determined as the most appropriate for our gray matter-based analytic methodology (36).

Statistical Analysis

Our primary goal was to study whether hippocampal volume was related to amyloid deposition in late-life depression. After data quality control procedures, 48 of 51 patients and 52 of 53 comparison subjects had both suitable PET and hippocampal volume data available for analysis (MRI data sets for three patients and one comparison subject were excluded because of motion artifacts). All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 23 (IBM, Armonk, N.Y.).

Group differences in hippocampal volume and amyloid binding.

We compared total hippocampal volume and SUVRcomp between patients with late-life depression and comparison subjects using a two-sample t test and a nonparametric Mann-Whitney U test, respectively (as SUVRs were nonnormally distributed). We assessed the proportion of positive to negative amyloid scans between groups using the predefined SUVRcomp threshold. To investigate hippocampal volume changes in the depression group relative to comparison subjects with subthreshold amyloid binding, we repeated these analyses excluding the amyloid-positive subjects in both groups. In addition, we ran an analysis of variance (ANOVA) to test for an interaction between depression diagnosis and amyloid status in the whole sample. Finally, we investigated differences in the association between amyloid binding, hippocampal volume, and white matter hyperintensity volume in patients and comparison subjects using both Spearman correlations and partial correlations (Pearson) controlling for age.

To examine an association with APOE genotype, we performed a multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) with hippocampal volume and SUVRcomp as dependent variables and APOE genotype (ε4 carrier status) and depression diagnosis as fixed factors.

Secondary analyses in patients.

To investigate any associations between regional amyloid binding in each cortical volume of interest and hippocampal volume, we used Spearman correlations and partial correlations controlling for age. To control for multiple comparisons, a Bonferroni correction was applied such that p values <0.01 were considered statistically significant (alpha=0.05/5 volumes of interest).

To examine potential associations between clinically relevant factors, hippocampal volume, and amyloid binding, we performed a series of exploratory MANCOVA analyses. These included 1) late versus early onset of depression (i.e., after or before age 55, respectively), 2) the presence of psychotic features, and 3) episodic memory performance (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test total learning score, delayed recall, and recognition [i.e., recognition minus false positives]). In all analyses, total hippocampal volume and SUVRcomp were dependent variables, and age was included as a covariate. To account for possible violations of the assumption of normality for SUVRcomp, we performed bootstrapping (1,000 samples) to assess the results. To control for multiple comparisons, p values <0.01 were considered statistically significant (alpha=0.05/5 MANCOVA analyses).

Results

Hippocampal volume was normally distributed, whereas SUVRcomp values were not. Participants’ demographic characteristics, APOE genotype, cognitive and clinical characteristics, and imaging data are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1. Demographic, Clinical, and Imaging Data for Patients With Late-Life Depression and Comparison Subjects

VariablePatient Group (N=48)Comparison Group (N=52)p
Demographic and clinical characteristics
MeanSDMeanSD
Age (years)a74.17.5172.46.430.230
N%N%
Female336937710.482
APOE genotype0.148
 ε2, ε20000
 ε2, ε3613917
 ε2, ε42412
 ε3, ε329602140
 ε3, ε410211937
 ε4, ε40024
 ε4 carrier122522420.061
MeanSDMeanSD
Mini-Mental State Examination254.17282.48<0.001
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
 Total learning2612.394511.67<0.001
 Delayed recall53.9693.45<0.001
 Recognition-false positives94.80123.28<0.001
Geriatric Depression Scale234.2833.14<0.001
N%
Psychotic depression1633
Late-onset depression2858
Imaging data
Amyloid positive at 1.38 threshold102112230.489
MedianMedian
SUVRcompb1.281.270.548
MeanSDMeanSD
Left hippocampal volume (mm3)3,1443863,3394160.017
Right hippocampal volume (mm3)3,2523643,3853810.077
Total intracranial volume (mL)1,3511321,3541530.897
White matter hyperintensity volume (mL)1.491.070.801.270.237

aThe age range was 61–88 years in the patient group and 59–86 years in the comparison group.

bSUVRcomp=composite cortical standardized uptake value ratio.

TABLE 1. Demographic, Clinical, and Imaging Data for Patients With Late-Life Depression and Comparison Subjects

Enlarge table

Group Differences in Hippocampal Volume and Amyloid Binding

Mean normalized total hippocampal volume was significantly lower in patients relative to comparison subjects (t=2.218, df=98, p=0.029; Cohen’s d=0.44), but there was no group difference in the distribution or median amyloid load. The hippocampal volume difference could be attributed to a decrease in both the left (t=2.423, df=98, p=0.017; d=0.485) and right (t=1.785, df=98, p=0.077; d=0.357) hippocampi (Figure 2). There was no difference in the proportion of participants with a positive versus a negative amyloid scan between groups at an SUVRcomp threshold of 1.38. A two-by-two factorial ANOVA with the between-subject factors depression diagnosis and amyloid status and hippocampal volume as outcome measure did not show a significant interaction.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2. Hippocampal Volume in Comparison Subjects and Patients With Late-Life Depression

a Statistically significant mean reduction in left hippocampal volume in patients (t=2.423, df=98, p=0.017; Cohen’s d=0.485). Note that the comparison subject with the lowest hippocampal volume was the oldest participant, age 88, who had a composite cortical standardized uptake value ratio (SUVRcomp) of 1.10.

Group differences in hippocampal volume excluding amyloid-positive subjects.

Below a threshold of 1.38, the difference in mean normalized total hippocampal volume between 38 amyloid-negative patients and 40 comparison subjects remained significant (t=2.145, df=76, p=0.035; d=0.486). There were no group differences in the distribution of SUVRcomp values (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3. Amyloid Deposition in Comparison Subjects and Patients With Late-Life Depression Based on Composite Standardized Uptake Value Ratio (SUVR) and the Five Regions It Is Derived Froma

a The dashed line represents an SUVR threshold of 1.38. There were no statistically significant differences in median SUVR, SUVR distribution, or the relative number of patients or comparison subjects who were amyloid positive.

Association between amyloid binding and hippocampal volume.

There were no significant associations between hippocampal volume and amyloid deposition in the composite volume of interest (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4. Correlation Between Total Hippocampal Volume and Amyloid Deposition in Comparison Subjects and Patients With Late-Life Depressiona

a The size of the data points represents age, with larger size representing older age. The dashed vertical lines represent the mean total hippocampal volume in patients and comparison subjects. Neither Pearson (partial) nor Spearman correlations were statistically significant.

b Significant difference between groups (t=2.218, df=98, p=0.029; Cohen’s d=0.44).

Association between white matter hyperintensities, hippocampal volume, and amyloid binding.

There was no association between total hippocampal volume and white matter hyperintensity volume in either patients or comparison subjects. Likewise, there was no association between amyloid load and white matter hyperintensity volume in either patients or comparison subjects.

Association between APOE genotype, amyloid binding, and hippocampal volume.

A main effect of APOE genotype was observed for amyloid load (F=14.350, df=98, p<0.001), but not for hippocampal volume. APOE ε4 carriers had a higher amyloid load than non-ε4 carriers. There was no diagnosis-by-APOE genotype interaction with regard to either amyloid load or hippocampal volume. Post hoc analysis in patients with late-life depression confirmed a main effect of APOE genotype for amyloid load (F=8.683, df=1, 46, p=0.005) but not for hippocampal volume.

Secondary Analyses in Patients

Regional associations between amyloid binding and hippocampal volume in patients.

There were no significant associations between amyloid load and hippocampal volume in any volume of interest as assessed by Spearman correlation coefficients and Pearson partial correlations controlling for age.

Associations between clinical factors, hippocampal volume, and amyloid binding in patients.

No main effects were detected for onset of depression or the presence of psychotic features. No main effects were detected for measures of episodic memory, including total learning, delayed recall, and delayed recognition.

Discussion

Our data confirm that hippocampal volume is smaller in patients with late-life depression relative to healthy comparison subjects. The main and novel finding of our study, however, is that lower hippocampal volume was not due to a higher degree of amyloid deposition in the late-life depression group. We did not find any correlation between hippocampal volume and amyloid deposition in patients, and hippocampal volume remained significantly lower when the comparison was restricted to amyloid-negative individuals. This finding was independent of the SUVRcomp threshold used (see the online data supplement). Similar to other studies in late-life depression (37), we found significant cognitive impairment, including episodic memory deficits, but no relationship between memory impairment and hippocampal volume or amyloid binding, suggesting that amyloid pathology may not be the cause of memory problems in our patient cohort. This finding supports neuropathological evidence showing that cognitive impairment in late-life depression is not associated with Alzheimer-type pathology (38). In agreement with other studies (4, 8), lower hippocampal volume in late-life depression was not associated with APOE ε4 carrier status despite our finding of higher amyloid binding in patients with APOE ε4 carrier status. Furthermore, there was no significant association between amyloid levels and onset of depression. Taken together, these findings suggest that Alzheimer’s pathology is unlikely to be the main cause of lower hippocampal volume in our patient cohort.

Although this study does not support Alzheimer’s disease as cause of hippocampal volume decrease in late-life depression, our findings do not necessarily refute the depression-as-late-life-neuropsychiatric-disorder model. It has recently been shown that hippocampal atrophy as a marker of neuronal injury may be present in the absence of amyloid pathology, a condition that has been called suspected nonamyloid pathology (39). Individuals with mild cognitive impairment associated with suspected nonamyloid pathology have an increased risk of cognitive deterioration comparable to that in individuals with mild cognitive impairment who have markers of neuronal injury in the presence of amyloid pathology (39). This could explain why patients with late-life depression who have hippocampal atrophy have an increased risk of developing dementia. Although vascular impairment may contribute to nonamyloid brain changes and the occurrence of late-life depressive symptoms, we did not find an association between white matter hyperintensities and hippocampal volume in our cohort.

Stress-related mechanisms were not the focus of this study, and we did not find evidence of significant hippocampal volume decrease in early-onset compared with late-onset depression, despite the absence of a relationship with amyloid pathology in the late-onset group. The lack of association with illness onset may be compatible with the vulnerability hypothesis, which predicts increased stress sensitivity and proneness to depression due to preexisting lower hippocampal volume (23).

As far as we know, this is the first prospective study using amyloid imaging in patients who were depressed at the time of scanning. In a small sample of remitted patients with late-life depression who underwent 11C-PIB PET (31), approximately half of the patients demonstrated increased amyloid binding. However, the majority of patients fulfilled the criteria for mild cognitive impairment after remission. A study examining the effect of a lifetime history of depression on cortical amyloid level using [18F]florbetapir in elderly subjects without cognitive impairment (30) showed increased amyloid binding in the precuneus and parietal cortex, but no global increases relative to comparison subjects and no association with age at onset. Another study using 11C-PIB PET in nondemented individuals with prior depressive episodes (29) did not find elevated cortical amyloid binding or specific changes in patients with late-onset depression relative to comparison subjects. However, remission as an inclusion criterion is associated with a selection bias and may not be representative of all patients with late-life depression.

Our findings may have important clinical implications. Differentiating between early Alzheimer’s disease and late-life depression is often challenging for clinicians. A significant proportion of depressed individuals have cognitive impairment, including episodic memory and attention deficits, overlapping with the cognitive profile of Alzheimer’s disease (40). In these circumstances, neuroimaging markers of neuronal injury, such as hippocampal atrophy, are commonly used to support a diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. However, hippocampal atrophy has mainly been validated as an imaging marker of Alzheimer’s disease in comparison with healthy subjects. In view of our data, hippocampal volume decrease as a diagnostic measure may be less reliable for assessing Alzheimer’s disease pathology in patients with late-life depression, compared with amyloid imaging.

Some limitations of the study need to be addressed. First, although the data used here are part of a longitudinal study, we only report cross-sectional findings because our objective was to determine whether hippocampal volume decrease was associated with amyloid levels at the time of diagnosis. However, it will be interesting to investigate what lower hippocampal volume in the absence of amyloid pathology means in terms of prognosis or risk of cognitive deterioration. Second, referral to our academic hospital may be associated with a selection bias toward more severe, therapy-resistant, and psychotic depressions or a lower likelihood of Alzheimer’s disease–related pathology according to the referring clinician. Finally, genetic stratification according to APOE status was present in some comparison subjects but not in the patient group, which could constitute a bias. However, we did not detect any group-by-APOE ε4 status interaction for hippocampal volume or SUVRcomp.

In summary, our study challenges the idea that lower hippocampal volume in late-life depression is due to Alzheimer’s disease and encourages the investigation of alternative models.

From the Department of Old Age Psychiatry, University Psychiatric Center, KU Leuven, Belgium; Translational MRI, Department of Imaging and Pathology, KU Leuven, and Radiology, University Hospitals Leuven; Research and Development, Icometrix, Leuven; GE Healthcare, London; University Psychiatric Center, KU Leuven, and Academic Center for ECT and Neuromodulation, Kortenberg, Belgium; Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, KU Leuven; the Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; the Department of Neurology, University Hospitals Leuven; Nuclear Medicine and Molecular Imaging, Department of Imaging and Pathology, KU Leuven and University Hospitals Leuven.
Address correspondence to Dr. Vandenbulcke ().

The first three authors contributed equally to this article.

European Union Drug Regulating Authorities Clinical Trials identifier: EudraCT 2009-018064-95.

Dr. Vandenbulcke has received funding from Research Foundation Flanders (FWO) (project G.0746.09). Dr. Vandenberghe is senior clinical research fellow for FWO and has received funding from the Foundation for Alzheimer Research (SAO-FRMA) (09013, 11020, 13007) and KU Leuven (OT/08/056, OT/12/097); he has received advisory panel payment from GE Healthcare, and his institution had a clinical trial agreement with GE Healthcare, Merck, Forum, and Eli Lilly. Dr. Van Laere is senior clinical research fellow for FWO and received contract research grants from GE Healthcare, Merck, and Janssen through KU Leuven. Dr. Adamczuk is a doctoral fellow of the Research Foundation Flanders. Dr. Farrar is an employee of GE Healthcare, London. Mr. Jain is an employee of Icometrix, Leuven. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests. [18F]Flutemetamol was provided by GE Healthcare free of charge for this academic investigator-driven trial.

References

1 Ashtari M, Greenwald BS, Kramer-Ginsberg E, et al.: Hippocampal/amygdala volumes in geriatric depression. Psychol Med 1999; 29:629–638Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Pantel J, Schröder J, Essig M, et al.: Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging in geriatric depression and primary degenerative dementia. J Affect Disord 1997; 42:69–83Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Lloyd AJ, Ferrier IN, Barber R, et al.: Hippocampal volume change in depression: late- and early-onset illness compared. Br J Psychiatry 2004; 184:488–495Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 O’Brien JT, Lloyd A, McKeith I, et al.: A longitudinal study of hippocampal volume, cortisol levels, and cognition in older depressed subjects. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:2081–2090LinkGoogle Scholar

5 Ballmaier M, Narr KL, Toga AW, et al.: Hippocampal morphology and distinguishing late-onset from early-onset elderly depression. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:229–237LinkGoogle Scholar

6 Gerritsen L, Comijs HC, van der Graaf Y, et al.: Depression, hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis, and hippocampal and entorhinal cortex volumes: the SMART Medea study. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70:373–380Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Steffens DC, Byrum CE, McQuoid DR, et al.: Hippocampal volume in geriatric depression. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:301–309Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Hickie I, Naismith S, Ward PB, et al.: Reduced hippocampal volumes and memory loss in patients with early- and late-onset depression. Br J Psychiatry 2005; 186:197–202Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Steffens DC, McQuoid DR, Payne ME, et al.: Change in hippocampal volume on magnetic resonance imaging and cognitive decline among older depressed and nondepressed subjects in the Neurocognitive Outcomes of Depression in the Elderly study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2011; 19:4–12Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Steffens DC, Payne ME, Greenberg DL, et al.: Hippocampal volume and incident dementia in geriatric depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002; 10:62–71Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Frodl T, O’Keane V: How does the brain deal with cumulative stress? A review with focus on developmental stress, HPA axis function, and hippocampal structure in humans. Neurobiol Dis 2013; 52:24–37Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Speck CE, Kukull WA, Brenner DE, et al.: History of depression as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease. Epidemiology 1995; 6:366–369Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Sweet RA, Hamilton RL, Butters MA, et al.: Neuropathologic correlates of late-onset major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:2242–2250Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Rapp MA, Schnaider-Beeri M, Grossman HT, et al.: Increased hippocampal plaques and tangles in patients with Alzheimer disease with a lifetime history of major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:161–167Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Tsopelas C, Stewart R, Savva GM, et al.: Neuropathological correlates of late-life depression in older people. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 198:109–114Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Wilson RS, Capuano AW, Boyle PA, et al.: Clinical-pathologic study of depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology 2014; 83:702–709Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 MacQueen G, Frodl T: The hippocampus in major depression: evidence for the convergence of the bench and bedside in psychiatric research? Mol Psychiatry 2011; 16:252–264Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Schmaal L, Veltman DJ, van Erp TG, et al.: Subcortical brain alterations in major depressive disorder: findings from the ENIGMA Major Depressive Disorder working group. Mol Psychiatry 2016; 21:806–812.Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Bell-McGinty S, Butters MA, Meltzer CC, et al.: Brain morphometric abnormalities in geriatric depression: long-term neurobiological effects of illness duration. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:1424–1427LinkGoogle Scholar

20 den Heijer T, Tiemeier H, Luijendijk HJ, et al.: A study of the bidirectional association between hippocampal volume on magnetic resonance imaging and depression in the elderly. Biol Psychiatry 2011; 70:191–197Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Janssen J, Hulshoff Pol HE, De Leeuw FE, et al.: Hippocampal volume and subcortical white matter lesions in late life depression: comparison of early and late onset depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007; 78:638–640Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Rao U, Chen LA, Bidesi AS, et al.: Hippocampal changes associated with early-life adversity and vulnerability to depression. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67:357–364Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, et al.: Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nat Neurosci 2002; 5:1242–1247Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Ownby RL, Crocco E, Acevedo A, et al.: Depression and risk for Alzheimer disease: systematic review, meta-analysis, and metaregression analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:530–538Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Jeong YH, Park CH, Yoo J, et al.: Chronic stress accelerates learning and memory impairments and increases amyloid deposition in APPV717I-CT100 transgenic mice, an Alzheimer’s disease model. FASEB J 2006; 20:729–731Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Peavy GM, Lange KL, Salmon DP, et al.: The effects of prolonged stress and APOE genotype on memory and cortisol in older adults. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:472–478Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Klunk WE, Engler H, Nordberg A, et al.: Imaging brain amyloid in Alzheimer’s disease with Pittsburgh Compound-B. Ann Neurol 2004; 55:306–319Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Vandenberghe R, Van Laere K, Ivanoiu A, et al.: 18F-Flutemetamol amyloid imaging in Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment: a phase 2 trial. Ann Neurol 2010; 68:319–329Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Madsen K, Hasselbalch BJ, Frederiksen KS, et al.: Lack of association between prior depressive episodes and cerebral [11C]PiB binding. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33:2334–2342Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Wu KY, Hsiao IT, Chen CS, et al.: Increased brain amyloid deposition in patients with a lifetime history of major depression: evidenced on 18F-florbetapir (AV-45/Amyvid) positron emission tomography. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 2014; 41:714–722Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Butters MA, Klunk WE, Mathis CA, et al.: Imaging Alzheimer pathology in late-life depression with PET and Pittsburgh Compound-B. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2008; 22:261–268Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Adamczuk K, De Weer AS, Nelissen N, et al.: Functional changes in the language network in response to increased amyloid β deposition in cognitively intact older adults. Cereb Cortex 2016; 26:358–373Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Jain S, Smeeets D, Loeckx D: Prior built population atlas for automatic hippocampus segmentation. ESMRMB 2013 Congress: Book of Abstracts, abstract 295 (doi: 10.1007/s10334-013-0383-6)Google Scholar

34 Boccardi M, Bocchetta M, Apostolova LG, et al.: Delphi definition of the EADC-ADNI Harmonized Protocol for hippocampal segmentation on magnetic resonance. Alzheimers Dement 2015; 11:126–138Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Jain S, Sima DM, Ribbens A, et al.: Automatic segmentation and volumetry of multiple sclerosis brain lesions from MR images. Neuroimage Clin 2015; 8:367–375Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Adamczuk K, Schaeverbeke J, Nelissen N, et al.: Amyloid imaging in cognitively normal older adults: comparison between (18)F-flutemetamol and (11)C-Pittsburgh compound B. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 2016; 43:142–151Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Butters MA, Whyte EM, Nebes RD, et al.: The nature and determinants of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:587–595Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 O’Brien J, Thomas A, Ballard C, et al.: Cognitive impairment in depression is not associated with neuropathologic evidence of increased vascular or Alzheimer-type pathology. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 49:130–136Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Caroli A, Prestia A, Galluzzi S, et al.: Mild cognitive impairment with suspected nonamyloid pathology (SNAP): Prediction of progression. Neurology 2015; 84:508–515Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Butters MA, Young JB, Lopez O, et al.: Pathways linking late-life depression to persistent cognitive impairment and dementia. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2008; 10:345–357MedlineGoogle Scholar