Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Issues
    • Issue in progress
    • Issues by date
  • Sections
    • Editorial
    • Review
    • Research
    • Commentary
    • Psychopharmacology for the Clinician
    • Letters to the Editor
  • Topic Collections
  • Instructions for Authors
    • Overview for authors
    • Submission checklist
    • Editorial policies
    • Publication fees
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Dr. Francis Wayne Quan Memorial Prize
    • Open access
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • About
    • General information
    • Staff
    • Editorial Board
    • Contact
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ
    • CMAJ Open
    • CJS
    • JAMC

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
JPN
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ
    • CMAJ Open
    • CJS
    • JAMC
JPN

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Issues
    • Issue in progress
    • Issues by date
  • Sections
    • Editorial
    • Review
    • Research
    • Commentary
    • Psychopharmacology for the Clinician
    • Letters to the Editor
  • Topic Collections
  • Instructions for Authors
    • Overview for authors
    • Submission checklist
    • Editorial policies
    • Publication fees
    • Submit a manuscript
    • Dr. Francis Wayne Quan Memorial Prize
    • Open access
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • About
    • General information
    • Staff
    • Editorial Board
    • Contact
  • Subscribe to our alerts
  • RSS feeds
  • Follow JPN on Twitter
Research Paper

Effects of dopaminergic genes, prenatal adversities, and their interaction on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and neural correlates of response inhibition

Dennis van der Meer, Catharina A. Hartman, Daan van Rooij, Barbara Franke, Dirk J. Heslenfeld, Jaap Oosterlaan, Stephen V. Faraone, Jan K. Buitelaar and Pieter J. Hoekstra
J Psychiatry Neurosci March 01, 2017 42 (2) 113-121; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.150350
Dennis van der Meer
From the University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen, The Netherlands (van der Meer, Hartman, Hoekstra); the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (van Rooij); the Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (Franke); the Clinical Neuropsychology Section, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan); the Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York, USA (Faraone); the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (Faraone); the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar); and the Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: [email protected]
Catharina A. Hartman
From the University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen, The Netherlands (van der Meer, Hartman, Hoekstra); the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (van Rooij); the Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (Franke); the Clinical Neuropsychology Section, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan); the Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York, USA (Faraone); the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (Faraone); the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar); and the Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Daan van Rooij
From the University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen, The Netherlands (van der Meer, Hartman, Hoekstra); the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (van Rooij); the Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (Franke); the Clinical Neuropsychology Section, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan); the Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York, USA (Faraone); the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (Faraone); the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar); and the Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Barbara Franke
From the University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen, The Netherlands (van der Meer, Hartman, Hoekstra); the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (van Rooij); the Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (Franke); the Clinical Neuropsychology Section, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan); the Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York, USA (Faraone); the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (Faraone); the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar); and the Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dirk J. Heslenfeld
From the University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen, The Netherlands (van der Meer, Hartman, Hoekstra); the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (van Rooij); the Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (Franke); the Clinical Neuropsychology Section, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan); the Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York, USA (Faraone); the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (Faraone); the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar); and the Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jaap Oosterlaan
From the University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen, The Netherlands (van der Meer, Hartman, Hoekstra); the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (van Rooij); the Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (Franke); the Clinical Neuropsychology Section, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan); the Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York, USA (Faraone); the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (Faraone); the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar); and the Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Stephen V. Faraone
From the University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen, The Netherlands (van der Meer, Hartman, Hoekstra); the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (van Rooij); the Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (Franke); the Clinical Neuropsychology Section, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan); the Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York, USA (Faraone); the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (Faraone); the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar); and the Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jan K. Buitelaar
From the University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen, The Netherlands (van der Meer, Hartman, Hoekstra); the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (van Rooij); the Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (Franke); the Clinical Neuropsychology Section, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan); the Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York, USA (Faraone); the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (Faraone); the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar); and the Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Pieter J. Hoekstra
From the University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, Department of Psychiatry, Groningen, The Netherlands (van der Meer, Hartman, Hoekstra); the Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (van Rooij); the Departments of Human Genetics and Psychiatry, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The Netherlands (Franke); the Clinical Neuropsychology Section, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan); the Departments of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York, USA (Faraone); the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway (Faraone); the Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar); and the Karakter Child and Adolescent Psychiatry University Centre, Nijmegen, Netherlands (Buitelaar)
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Tables
  • Related Content
  • Responses
  • Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Background: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is often accompanied by impaired response inhibition; both have been associated with aberrant dopamine signalling. Given that prenatal exposure to alcohol or smoking is known to affect dopamine-rich brain regions, we hypothesized that individuals carrying the ADHD risk alleles of the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) and dopamine transporter (DAT1) genes may be especially sensitive to their effects.

Methods: Functional MRI data, information on prenatal adversities and genetic data were available for 239 adolescents and young adults participating in the multicentre ADHD cohort study NeuroIMAGE (average age 17.3 yr). We analyzed the effects of DRD4 and DAT1, prenatal exposure to alcohol and smoking and their interactions on ADHD severity, response inhibition and neural activity.

Results: We found no significant gene × environment interaction effects. We did find that the DRD4 7-repeat allele was associated with less superior frontal and parietal brain activity and with greater activity in the frontal pole and occipital cortex. Prenatal exposure to smoking was also associated with lower superior frontal activity, but with greater activity in the parietal lobe. Further, those exposed to alcohol had more activity in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, and the DAT1 risk variant was associated with lower cerebellar activity.

Limitations: Retrospective reports of maternal substance use and the cross-sectional study design restrict causal inference.

Conclusion: While we found no evidence of gene × environment interactions, the risk factors under investigation influenced activity of brain regions associated with response inhibition, suggesting they may add to problems with inhibiting behaviour.

Introduction

Many individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have impaired response inhibition (i.e., a compromised ability to suppress inappropriate responses).1 Functional neuroimaging studies of response inhibition have typically implicated 2 networks. A frontostriatal network consisting of the inferior frontal gyrus, presupplementary motor area and basal ganglia is involved in controlling and executing the response inhibition process,2 and a frontoparietal network consisting of the superior frontal gyrus and parietal lobe has been suggested to be important for attentional processing and top–down control of behaviour.3 Previous work from our group has shown that individuals with ADHD have problems engaging both these networks.4

Both ADHD and response inhibition are highly heritable5,6 and share genetic underpinnings.5 The most frequently investigated candidate genes for ADHD, the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) and the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1, or SLC6A3), have also been associated with response inhibition performance and related brain activity.7,8 DRD4 contains a variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) polymorphism in exon 3, for which the 7-repeat (7R) variant is considered a risk factor for ADHD. In vitro studies have indicated that the protein product of this gene, mainly expressed in the frontal cortex, may be less sensitive to dopamine if the 7R allele is present.9 This agrees with studies reporting lower activity across the frontal lobe for 7R carriers during tasks reliant on cognitive processes, including response inhibition.10 This involves brain regions affected in individuals with ADHD.11 DAT1 contains 2 VNTRs that have been repeatedly linked to ADHD, 1 in the 3′ untranslated region (3′ UTR, 10R risk allele for childhood ADHD, 9R risk allele for adult ADHD) and 1 in intron 8 (i8; 6R risk allele for both childhood and adult ADHD), albeit with substantial heterogeneity between studies.12 A haplotype of these 2 VNTRs has shown a stronger association with ADHD than either VNTR analyzed separately, both in children13 and in persistent, adult ADHD.14 The dopamine transporter (DAT1) is most strongly expressed in striatal regions,15 and positron emission tomography studies have provided highly significant evidence that the 9R allele is associated with increased DAT binding in striatal brain regions.16 Neuroimaging studies have indicated that children with ADHD homozygous for the DAT1 3′ UTR 10R risk allele have reduced volumes of the caudate nucleus and lowered neural activation of this region during response inhibition.17,18 However, some reports have claimed effects of DAT1 on the caudate nucleus and cortical regions involved in response inhibition in the opposite direction,19 suggesting that its effects on brain and behaviour may depend on other factors.

An unfavourable prenatal environment has also been linked to an increased risk for ADHD.20 Individuals exposed in utero to either alcohol or cigarette smoking show a range of cognitive impairments, including lowered executive functioning and attention as well as higher levels of hyperactivity and impulsive behaviour.21 Prenatal exposure to both alcohol and smoking has been associated with hypoactive dopaminergic neurotransmission22,23 and with lowered activity and structure in the dopamine-rich frontal cortex and basal ganglia,24 which are thought to be central to response inhibition.2

Evidence is accumulating that gene × environment interactions are involved in shaping the ADHD phenotype.25 Several studies have reported that individuals carrying dopaminergic genotypes associated with ADHD are more sensitive to the effects of prenatal exposure to alcohol and smoking than individuals without these genetic risk factors.26–28 However, other reports with null findings have led to skepticism regarding the existence of these gene × environment interactions.29,30 To our knowledge, no study to date has used functional neuroimaging data to investigate the interplay between the dopaminergic genetic and environmental factors at the neural level. This may provide information on potential neural mechanisms and thereby give biological validity to the reported interaction effects.

In this study, we aimed to shed light on the neural correlates of the most prominent dopaminergic candidate genes for ADHD, prenatal exposure to alcohol or smoking and their interaction during a response inhibition task in a large sample of adolescents and young adults with and without ADHD. We hypothesized that all risk factors would be associated with lower activation of the dopamine-rich response inhibition network nodes. Based on previous neuroimaging findings,18,31 we postulated that the DAT1 risk haplotype may convey higher sensitivity to prenatal exposure to alcohol or smoking on the striatum and associated dopaminergic brain regions that control and execute response inhibition,2 while the lowered dopamine signalling associated with the DRD4 7R allele may cause carriers to be more reactive to the effects of prenatal stressors on the frontal, cortical nodes of the response inhibition networks10 that are responsible for top–down control.3

Methods

Participants and protocol

Participants were part of the NeuroIMAGE study, a follow-up of the Dutch part of the International Multicentre ADHD Genetics (IMAGE) study.32 NeuroIMAGE includes 365 families with at least 1 child with ADHD and at least 1 biological sibling (regardless of ADHD diagnosis) as well as 148 control families with at least 1 child without any formal or suspected ADHD diagnosis in any of the first-degree family members. The ADHD families were recruited through ADHD outpatient clinics in the regions Amsterdam, Groningen and Nijmegen (The Netherlands). Control families were recruited through primary and high schools in the same geographical regions to match the ADHD sample. To be included in NeuroIMAGE, participants had to be between 5 and 30 years old and of white European descent; have an IQ of 70 or higher; and have no diagnosis of autism, epilepsy, general learning difficulties, brain disorders, or known genetic disorders. More information on the NeuroIMAGE study and its participants is available elsewhere.32

The 239 participants who met the NeuroIMAGE inclusion criteria had data available on prenatal stress and genotype and who had completed the fMRI task came from 148 families; 89 participants from 71 families had a diagnosis of ADHD, and 150 were unaffected controls (63 of whom were siblings of participants with ADHD).

All measurements were part of a comprehensive assessment protocol. Testing was carried out either at the VU University Amsterdam and VU University Medical Center (VU UMC) or at the Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre and Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour in Nijmegen. Participants were motivated with short breaks and received €50 and a copy of their MRI scan at the end of the day. The study was approved by the regional ethics committee (CMO Regio Arnhem — Nijmegen; 2008/163; ABR: NL23894.091.08), and the medical ethical committee of the VU University Medical Center. All participants signed informed consent (parents signed informed consent for participants younger than 12 years).

Assessment of ADHD

The ADHD diagnoses were made in accordance with DSM IV-TR criteria on the basis of a combination of a semistructured diagnostic interview, the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia — Present and Lifetime version33 and the Conners Rating Scales.34 We constructed an ADHD symptom count based on the Conners ADHD Rating Scales questionnaires. These questionnaires were filled in by the parents and either a teacher (for children < 18 yr) or the participants themselves (for those ≥ 18 yr). The Conners Rating Scales provide operational definitions of each of the 18 ADHD symptoms defined in the DSM-IV-TR. In this sample, the symptom count ranged from 0 to 18 with an average of 4.6. Thirty-five participants had an oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder, 8 an internalizing disorder and 27 had a reading disorder. An extensive description of the diagnostic algorithm for ADHD and comorbid disorders is provided in Appendix 1, available at jpn.ca.

Assessment of prenatal exposure to alcohol and cigarette smoking

Information on prenatal exposure to maternal alcohol use and smoking was obtained retrospectively using a structured questionnaire that was filled out by the parents at IMAGE. The questionnaire was derived from the Prechtl optimality scales.35 For both prenatal exposure to alcohol and smoking, a code of 0 indicated the absence of the risk factor, and a code of 1 indicated presence of the risk factor.

Genetic data

Genotyping was performed as described previously.36 Briefly, DNA was extracted from blood samples at Rutgers University Cell and DNA Repository, New Jersey, USA, or at the Department of Human Genetics of the Radboud University Medical Centre. Standard polymerase chain reaction protocols were used for the determination of the DRD4 and DAT1 genotype.

For the VNTR in exon 3 of DRD4, we used a dominant genetic model for the 7R allele, comparing 7R carriers to all others, in accordance with the majority of studies of this polymorphism.12 We calculated a haplotype of the alleles of the DAT1 3′ UTR and intron 8 VNTRs using the Haplostats package in R software version 3.1.1.37,38 For this haplotype, we compared 10–6 homozygotes with all others. Compliance of genotype distribution of DRD4, DAT1 3′ UTR and DAT1 i8 with Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium was checked using standard methods.

Stop-signal task

We used the stop-signal task to measure response inhibition; this task has been extensively described elsewhere.39 Participants were asked to respond with a button press as quickly as possible when a stimulus was presented, the so-called “go” signal. In a subset of trials, the go signal was followed after a short interval by a stop signal, indicating that the participant had to withhold the response. The delay between the go and stop signals was either increased or decreased by 50 ms after each stop trial, depending on success or failure to inhibit the button press. This allowed for calculation of the time the participant needs to successfully withhold a response in approximately 50% of the trials, known as the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT). As task performance measures, we looked at the SSRT (measuring speed of the inhibitory process), reaction time variability (RTV; measuring variability of performance) and the number of errors on go trials.

MRI data acquisition and analysis

The MRI data were acquired at both sites with similar 1.5 T Siemens scanners (Siemens Sonata at VU UMC in Amsterdam; Siemens Avanto at Donders Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging in Nijmegen) using identical protocols. The data were processed using FSL FEAT (FMRIB Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl; fMRI Expert Analysis Tool, version 6.0). Further details on MRI data acquisition and preprocessing can be found in Appendix 1.

Our approach to first-level analysis is identical to that of Van Rooij and colleagues.40 We constructed general linear models for each participant, containing regressors for fMRI blood-oxygen level–dependent (BOLD) responses to successful stop, failed stop and successful go trials. Failed go trials, signal from cerebral spinal fluid and white matter, and 24 realignment parameters were included as covariates.

We investigated the neural correlates of response inhibition by specifying a successful stop–go and a failed stop–go contrast. In other words, we used go trial activity as an implicit baseline to isolate activation evoked by stop trials. This approach is based on the fact that stop trials are identical to go trials, up to the stop signal. As all the preparatory processes are the same, the activity captured by these contrasts is assumed to reflect the inhibition process. This is substantiated in Appendix 1, Figure S1, which shows these 2 contrasts are associated with patterns of activity specifically in brain regions known to contribute to response inhibition.

We further isolated activity specific to failed trials compared with successful trials, through a failed – successful stop contrast. As the required response time was adjusted after each trial based on its outcome, as described, this contrast does not directly reflect brain activity associated with absolute speed of inhibition but rather brain activity when the participant is unable to handle task demands.

For group-level analysis we made use of the FSL tool PALM, which performs inference through permutation.41 To take into account the family relatedness present in the sample, we specified exchangeability blocks based on family membership, which ensured that only the rearrangements of the data that respect exchangeability were used. The data were permuted 5000 times, and significant effects were identified through threshold-free cluster enhancement, controlling the family-wise error (FWE) rate resulting from whole-brain analysis.42 We first evaluated possible main effects of each gene or prenatal stressor separately, followed by 4 models, one for each combination of gene and prenatal stressor and their interaction. In addition to the variables of interest, we added age, sex and scanner location to these models as covariates. All continuous predictors were mean-centred. Given that we ran multiple analyses with 4 risk factors and their interactions, we report only those clusters surviving a significance threshold of p = 0.005, FWE-corrected. Localization was carried out using the Harvard–Oxford atlas. All reported coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space in millimetres.

All analyses except the whole-brain fMRI analyses were carried out using R software version 3.1.1.37 Differences in sample demographics between groups based on presence versus absence of the risk factors under investigation were checked using Pearson χ2 tests for categorical variables and with 1-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for continuous variables. As with the fMRI analyses described previously, we ran models investigating the effect of DRD4, DAT1, prenatal exposure to alcohol, or prenatal exposure to smoking as well as the 4 different gene × environment combinations. All models included age, sex and location as covariates. In order to account for the within-family correlation due to the inclusion of siblings in the sample, we analyzed the data with linear mixed-effects models with family as a random factor, estimating a random intercept. The p values of the mixed models were estimated using a Markov chain Monte Carlo algorithm included in the languageR package. Given the multiple testing, we considered results to be significant at p ≤ 0.005. Significance of the post hoc mediation analysis was determined through bootstrapping with 5000 samples.43

Sensitivity analyses

We conducted sensitivity analyses to check whether the findings were driven by specific groups of participants by rerunning the analyses within diagnostic groups (ADHD, controls), testing locations (Amsterdam, Nijmegen), parental education levels (high, low) and age groups (adults, children). More information on the methods for these analyses can be found in Appendix 1.

Results

Demographic characteristics

Demographic characteristics of the study sample as a whole and per risk factor are displayed in Table 1. Individuals exposed prenatally to alcohol or smoking were on average older. The parents of those exposed to smoking had lower levels of education, whereas the parents of those exposed to alcohol had higher levels of education, in accordance with previously reported patterns.44 No differences between risk factor carriers and noncarriers were found in distribution of sex or scanning location. We found no evidence of gene–environment correlations, as displayed in Table 1. Genotype frequencies did not deviate from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (DRD4, p = 0.37; DAT1 3′ UTR, p = 0.19; DAT1 i8, p = 0.62).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Characteristics of the study sample split by risk factor*

Association of risk factors with ADHD and task performance

There were no main effects of the genotypes or environmental factors on ADHD severity. We also found no evidence that DRD4 or DAT1 genotype moderated the association of exposure to alcohol or smoking with ADHD severity (Table 2).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2

Results from the regression analyses of the effects of the risk factors on ADHD symptom count and response inhibition task performance

The association of ADHD with task performance in the NeuroIMAGE sample has been described in detail elsewhere.4 As in that report, in the present subset of participants, higher ADHD symptom count was associated with impairment on all 3 performance measures: more RTV, more errors and marginally longer SSRTs (Table 2).

Analysis of the association between the risk factors under investigation and task performance yielded 1 significant result: prenatal exposure to smoking was associated with higher RTV (B = 21.51 ± 6.31, p < 0.001). We found no significant association between prenatal exposure to alcohol or the genes and task performance or any evidence that DAT1 or DRD4 modulated the effect of prenatal exposure to alcohol or smoking (Table 2).

Association of the risk factors with neural correlates of response inhibition

The task activity maps associated with the 3 contrasts of interest are displayed in Appendix 1, Figure S1. These contrasts showed strong activation in the frontal and parietal regions commonly considered to be part of response inhibition networks.4

The DRD4 7R carriers showed lower activation of the superior and middle frontal gyrus than noncarriers during successful stop trials and less activation of the left superior parietal lobe during failed trials. Prenatal exposure to smoking was also associated with lower activation of the superior frontal gyrus extending into the anterior cingulate during successful trials, yet with greater activation in the superior parietal lobe and supramarginal gyrus during failed trials. Prenatal alcohol exposure was associated with more activation in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) during failed trials. There were no effects of the DAT1 haplotype or any interaction effects for either of the 2 types of stop trials relative to go trials.

When contrasting failed with successful stop trials, DAT1 risk haplotype homozygotes showed lower cerebellar activation than others. Further, DRD4 7R carriers had more activation of the lateral occipital cortex and frontal medial cortex than noncarriers, and prenatal exposure to smoking was associated with relatively more activation of the supramarginal gyrus bilaterally. Table 3 provides more details on all clusters found.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3

Clusters where neural activation correlated with the risk factors under investigation for the 3 fMRI contrasts

Post hoc analysis

Given that prenatal exposure to smoking was significantly associated with RTV, we checked whether the neural activation patterns associated with exposure to smoking explained this behavioural effect. We extracted the mean BOLD response for the significant clusters and found that activity during successful inhibition trials in the superior frontal and anterior cingulate cluster was significantly correlated with RTV (B = −0.96 ± 0.24, p < 0.001). Combined with the association between exposure to smoking and activity in this cluster (Table 3), there was a mediation effect (B = 6.74 ± 3.20, p = 0.030). In other words, activity in the superior frontal and anterior cingulate gyrus during successful response inhibition explained part of the association between prenatal exposure to smoking and RTV. The cluster in the left parietal lobe, found during failed inhibition, did not show a significant association with RTV (B = 0.008 ± 0.20, p = 0.97), nor did the clusters in the left (B = −1.41 ± 2.69, p = 0.60) or right (B = 6.84 ± 3.86, p = 0.08) supramarginal gyrus for the failed – successful stop contrast.

Sensitivity analyses

Directions and strength of significant effects reported in the main analyses were similar for those with and without an ADHD diagnosis, those tested in Amsterdam and in Nijmegen, children of parents with low and high education levels, and those younger and older than 18 years (Appendix 1, Table S2).

Discussion

In the present study, we investigated whether prenatal exposure to alcohol or smoking affected response inhibition and associated brain activity in a sample of adolescents and young adults with and without ADHD. We further analyzed whether genetic variation in DRD4 or DAT1 was associated with the behavioural and brain measures and whether this variation modulated the effects of the prenatal stressors, given previously reported interactions between these genetic and environmental factors.

We found that ADHD severity was not associated with prenatal adversities, genetic variation or gene × environment effects. Findings on the association between these risk factors and ADHD have been heterogeneous,25 indicating that any effects they may have on neurobiology are not strong enough to be consistently visible at the behavioural level. This may be because their neural correlates are only partially overlapping with the neurobiological pathways underlying the ADHD phenotype in most individuals, in line with suggestions of the existence of neurobiological subtypes of ADHD.45 The effects of prenatal exposure to a stressor are likely to change over time due to brain development and organization, which may also contribute to inconsistency of findings in the literature. The genetic variation in DAT1 and DRD4 is known to influence dopaminergic neurotransmission,9,16 central in adapting brain circuitry due to learning and experience.46 Our sensitivity analyses showed similar effects of the risk factors in both children and adults, though longitudinal neuroimaging studies are required to properly investigate to what extent these factors have an impact on the brain’s developmental trajectory.

While we found no association with our behavioural outcome measures, DRD4 7R carriers did show lower activity than noncarriers in a large cluster across the frontal lobe during successful response inhibition trials and lower activity in the superior parietal lobe during failed response inhibition. The 7R variant is thought to be less sensitive to dopamine signals than other variants,9 which may explain the lower activity of cortical regions where DRD4 is most strongly expressed.47 We further found that the 7R allele is associated with relatively greater activity in the lateral occipital cortex and frontal pole during failed trials compared with successful trials. Together with the superior parietal lobe, these regions are thought to integrate previous knowledge and expectations in order to optimize performance through attentional shifts.48 As failed inhibition signals a need to update expectations, it makes sense these regions are more active during failed trials than during successful trials. Although speculative, this pattern of findings may suggest that 7R carriers use a different cognitive strategy characterized by lower frontoparietal network activity and greater activity in medial-frontal and posterior brain regions, conveying greater reactivity to unexpected stimuli. This may also contribute to findings of differential susceptibility by DRD4 7R carriers to (prenatal) environmental factors.49

Exposure to maternal smoking in utero was also associated with lower brain activation in the superior frontal gyrus during successful response inhibition, yet with higher activation in the superior parietal lobe during failed trials. As the significant post hoc mediation analysis suggested, insufficient recruitment of the frontal lobe may lead to attentional lapses reflected in greater variability of response times, in line with results from lesion studies.50 One possibility is that, for those exposed prenatally to smoking, the observed increased activity of the parietal lobe reflects adaptation in response to lowered frontal lobe function. Johnson and colleagues51 have recently argued there is a central role of adaptation in the developing human brain in the face of environmental insults (e.g., through reorganization of the networks underlying attentional control). This coincides with previous reports that ADHD is characterized by a more diffuse pattern of brain activation during tasks taxing response inhibition.52,53

The striking overlap of the neural correlates of DRD4 with those of prenatal exposure to smoking found in the present study may underlie previously reported interaction effects between these risk factors on ADHD.27 Whereas the effect of either factor on the superior frontal gyrus by itself may not be strong enough to become visible as behaviour associated with ADHD, their combined effect may lower frontal lobe function to an extent that cannot be compensated by heightened activity in other brain regions, leading to impaired response inhibition. The opposite effects of DRD4 and prenatal exposure to smoking on the parietal lobe may also be involved in this process; if those individuals exposed to smoking in utero require greater activity of this brain region to compensate for lower frontal brain activity, then the lower dopamine sensitivity conveyed by the DRD4 7R allele may hinder upregulation in individuals carrying both risk factors. This reasoning is motivated by reports that DRD4 is involved in adjusting long-term synaptic strength and activity in response to stimuli,54 and weaker executive network connectivity displayed by 7R carriers.11 While these are currently speculations, they serve to show that neural data can be used to identify potential mechanisms underlying gene × environment interaction effects on behaviour.

We further observed that prenatal alcohol exposure was associated with more activity in the OFC during failed response inhibition trials. This region has been coupled to impulsivity and motivation processes55 known to be affected both in individuals with prenatal exposure to alcohol56 and those with ADHD.57 The lateral OFC is important for adaptive learning and has recently been linked to conflict adaptation during response inhibition.58 However, the meaning of these findings requires further study, as we found no effects of alcohol exposure on our behavioural outcome measures.

Finally, while we found no association between DAT1 variation and ADHD severity or response inhibition performance, homozygotes for the risk haplotype did show lower activity in the cerebellum during failed trials relative to successful trials. This fits well with the work of Durston and colleagues45 reporting lowered cerebellar activity during a response inhibition task in a group of individuals with ADHD and their siblings, specifically during trials that were temporally unpredictable.45,59 These authors also showed that cerebellar activity was modulated by DAT1 genotype.17 The lack of behavioural correlates, despite the reported effect on the cerebellum, add to the literature indicating that DAT1 has a complex relation with ADHD etiology, with previous studies reporting contradictory findings or no association at both the behavioural12 and the brain level.18 It has been posited that in persistent ADHD the 3′ UTR 9R allele rather than the 10R allele is associated with ADHD.14 Our sample spans a wide age range, from childhood to young adulthood, and is likely to contain a mixture of persistent and remitting ADHD, which may obscure the effects of the DAT1 genotype. Future studies better suited to investigate persistent ADHD are needed to investigate this possibility.

Limitations

To our knowledge, this study is the first to investigate possible moderating effects of dopaminergic genes on prenatal adversity at the neural level. We also used a large sample size compared with previous neuroimaging studies of prenatal stressors. However, null findings may still be attributed to a lack of statistical power given the small effect sizes reported for these genetic variants12 and the known power issues in gene × environment interaction analyses.60 Further, as with the majority of previous studies into prenatal adversity, we relied on retrospective reports of maternal substance use, which may have reduced accuracy.61 An additional important caveat of the results from this observational study is that the reported behavioural and neural correlates of exposure to alcohol and smoking in humans are likely to be confounded with other factors. A recently conducted study reported that the association between ADHD and prenatal exposure to smoking disappeared when controlling for genetic influences through the use of half- and full siblings in the study design. The authors stated the association between ADHD and prenatal exposure to smoking might result from a link between the mother’s genetic makeup and her substance use.62 The DRD4 7R allele, for instance, has been associated both with ADHD12 and with lowered success in abstaining from smoking.63 These findings show how maternal genotype may increase both the odds of prenatal exposure to smoking and onset of ADHD, which would contribute to an association between them in their offspring. This illustrates just one of many possible mechanisms of how suspected genetic and environmental risk factors may be intertwined, obscuring their true effects. Observational studies therefore need to be complemented by designs that allow for causal inference. Animal experimental studies have provided causal evidence that prenatal exposure to ethanol or nicotine has detrimental effects on the developing brain, particularly the proper formation of dopaminergic circuits.64 Genetically sensitive designs, such as those taking advantage of varying degrees of relatedness between participants,65 can further aid in estimating the causal effects in humans.

Conclusion

Our findings do not provide evidence for gene × environment interactions between prenatal stressors and dopaminergic genes on ADHD or response inhibition task performance. While we found no direct evidence, our data suggest that prenatal stressors can add to problems with response inhibition by affecting recruitment of underlying brain regions. Lower dopamine signalling conveyed by genetic variation may further exacerbate these effects, contributing to gene × environment interaction effects in behavioural studies. These results not only help further our understanding of the biological pathways associated with prenatal substance exposure, but also may help to indicate who is most sensitive to their effects.

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge the Department of Pediatrics of the VU University Medical Center for having the opportunity to use the mock scanner for preparation of the participants. This work was supported by NIH Grant R01MH62873 (to S.V. Faraone), NWO Large Investment Grant 1750102007010 and NWO Brain & Cognition an Integrative Approach grant (433-09-242) (to J. Buitelaar), and grants from Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, University Medical Center Groningen and Accare, and VU University Amsterdam. The research leading to these results also received funding from the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreement numbers 278948 (TACTICS), 602450 (IMAGEMEND), and n° 602805 (Aggressotype), and from the European Community’s Horizon 2020 Programme (H2020/2014 – 2020) under grant agreement n° 643051 (MiND). B. Franke is supported by a Vici grant from NWO (grant number 016-130-669). In addition, J. Buitelaar and B. Franke are supported by a grant for the ENIGMA Consortium (grant number U54 EB020403) from the BD2K Initiative of a cross-NIH partnership. S. Faraone is supported by the K.G. Jebsen Centre for Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway, the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreement n°602805 and NIMH grant R01MH094469.

Footnotes

  • Competing interests: B. Franke declares receiving speaker fees from Merz Pharma. J. Buitelaar declares receiving speaker fees from Lilly, Janssen Cilag BV, Shire, Medice, Servier, Roche and Lundbeck. P. Hoekstra declares receiving a grant and personal fees from Shire. No other competing interests declared.

  • Contributors: All authors designed the study. D. van Rooij acquired and analyzed the data, which D. van der Meer also analyzed. D. van der Meer wrote the article, which all authors reviewed and approved for publication.

  • Received November 11, 2015.
  • Revision received May 24, 2016.
  • Accepted July 5, 2016.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Alderson RM,
    2. Rapport MD,
    3. Kofler MJ
    .Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and behavioral inhibition: a meta-analytic review of the stop-signal paradigm.J Abnorm Child Psychol 2007;35:745–58.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Aron AR
    .From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses.Biol Psychiatry 2011;69:e55–68.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Fassbender C,
    2. Simoes-Franklin C,
    3. Murphy K,
    4. et al
    .The role of a right fronto-parietal network in cognitive control.J Psychophysiol 2006;20:286–96.
    OpenUrl
  4. ↵
    1. van Rooij D,
    2. Hartman CA,
    3. Mennes M,
    4. et al
    .Altered neural connectivity during response inhibition in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings.Neuroimage Clin 2015;7:325–35.
    OpenUrl
  5. ↵
    1. Crosbie J,
    2. Arnold P,
    3. Paterson A,
    4. et al
    .Response inhibition and ADHD traits: correlates and heritability in a community sample.J Abnorm Child Psychol 2013;41:497–507.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    1. Faraone SV,
    2. Perlis RH,
    3. Doyle AE,
    4. et al
    .Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:1313–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    1. Congdon E,
    2. Lesch KP,
    3. Canli T
    .Analysis of DRD4 and DAT polymorphisms and behavioral inhibition in healthy adults: implications for impulsivity.Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2008;147B:27–32.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  8. ↵
    1. Albrecht B,
    2. Brandeis D,
    3. Uebel-von Sandersleben H,
    4. et al
    .Genetics of preparation and response control in ADHD: the role of DRD4 and DAT1.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2014;55:914–23.
    OpenUrl
  9. ↵
    1. Asghari V,
    2. Sanyal S,
    3. Buchwaldt S,
    4. et al
    .Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants.J Neurochem 1995;65:1157–65.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    1. Mulligan RC,
    2. Kristjansson SD,
    3. Reiersen AM,
    4. et al
    .Neural correlates of inhibitory control and functional genetic variation in the dopamine D4 receptor gene.Neuropsychologia 2014;62:306–18.
    OpenUrl
  11. ↵
    1. Gilsbach S,
    2. Neufang S,
    3. Scherag S,
    4. et al
    .Effects of the DRD4 genotype on neural networks associated with executive functions in children and adolescents.Dev Cogn Neurosci 2012;2:417–27.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Gizer IR,
    2. Ficks C,
    3. Waldman ID
    .Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review.Hum Genet 2009;126:51–90.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Asherson P,
    2. Brookes K,
    3. Franke B,
    4. et al
    .Confirmation that a specific haplotype of the dopamine transporter gene is associated with combined-type ADHD.Am J Psychiatry 2007;164:674–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. Franke B,
    2. Vasquez AA,
    3. Johansson S,
    4. et al
    .Multicenter analysis of the SLC6A3/DAT1 VNTR haplotype in persistent ADHD suggests differential involvement of the gene in childhood and persistent ADHD.Neuropsychopharmacology 2010;35:656–664.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. ↵
    1. Ciliax BJ,
    2. Drash GW,
    3. Staley JK,
    4. et al
    .Immunocytochemical localization of the dopamine transporter in human brain.J Comp Neurol 1999;409:38–56.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    1. Faraone SV,
    2. Spencer TJ,
    3. Madras BK,
    4. et al
    .Functional effects of dopamine transporter gene genotypes on in vivo dopamine transporter functioning: a meta-analysis.Mol Psychiatry 2014;19:880–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    1. Durston S,
    2. Fossella JA,
    3. Mulder MJ,
    4. et al
    .Dopamine transporter genotype conveys familial risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder through striatal activation.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008;47:61–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. ↵
    1. Durston S
    .Imaging genetics in ADHD.Neuroimage 2010;53:832–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    1. Bédard AC,
    2. Schulz KP,
    3. Cook EH Jr.,
    4. et al
    .Dopamine transporter gene variation modulates activation of striatum in youth with ADHD.Neuroimage 2010;53:935–42.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    1. Mick E,
    2. Biederman J,
    3. Faraone SV,
    4. et al
    .Case-control study of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and maternal smoking, alcohol use, and drug use during pregnancy.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;41:378–85.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Huizink AC,
    2. Mulder EJ
    .Maternal smoking, drinking or cannabis use during pregnancy and neurobehavioral and cognitive functioning in human offspring.Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30:24–41.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Ernst M,
    2. Moolchan ET,
    3. Robinson ML
    .Behavioral and neural consequences of prenatal exposure to nicotine.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001;40:630–41.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  23. ↵
    1. Shen R-Y,
    2. Hannigan JH,
    3. Kapatos G
    .Prenatal ethanol reduces the activity of adult midbrain dopamine neurons.Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1999;23:1801–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. ↵
    1. Derauf C,
    2. Kekatpure M,
    3. Neyzi N,
    4. et al
    .Neuroimaging of children following prenatal drug exposure.Semin Cell Dev Biol 2009;20:441–54.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Nigg J,
    2. Nikolas M,
    3. Burt SA
    .Measured gene-by-environment interaction in relation to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010;49:863–73.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  26. ↵
    1. Brookes KJ,
    2. Mill J,
    3. Guindalini C,
    4. et al
    .A common haplotype of the dopamine transporter gene associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and interacting with maternal use of alcohol during pregnancy.Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63:74–81.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    1. Neuman RJ,
    2. Lobos E,
    3. Reich W,
    4. et al
    .Prenatal smoking exposure and dopaminergic genotypes interact to cause a severe ADHD subtype.Biol Psychiatry 2007;61:1320–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Kahn RS,
    2. Khoury J,
    3. Nichols WC,
    4. et al
    .Role of dopamine transporter genotype and maternal prenatal smoking in childhood hyperactive-impulsive, inattentive, and oppositional behaviors.J Pediatr 2003;143:104–10.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  29. ↵
    1. Langley K,
    2. Turic D,
    3. Rice F,
    4. et al
    .Testing for gene × environment interaction effects in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and associated antisocial behavior.Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2008;147B:49–53.
    OpenUrl
  30. ↵
    1. Altink ME,
    2. Arias-Vasquez A,
    3. Franke B,
    4. et al
    .The dopamine receptor D4 7-repeat allele and prenatal smoking in ADHD-affected children and their unaffected siblings: no gene-environment interaction.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008;49:1053–60.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  31. ↵
    1. Durston S,
    2. Fossella JA,
    3. Casey BJ,
    4. et al
    .Differential effects of DRD4 and DAT1 genotype on fronto-striatal gray matter volumes in a sample of subjects with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, their unaffected siblings, and controls.Mol Psychiatry 2005;10:678–85.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    1. von Rhein D,
    2. Mennes M,
    3. van Ewijk H,
    4. et al
    .The NeuroIMAGE study: a prospective phenotypic, cognitive, genetic and MRI study in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Design and descriptives.Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2015;24:265–81.
    OpenUrl
  33. ↵
    1. Kaufman J,
    2. Birmaher B,
    3. Brent D,
    4. et al
    .Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997;36:980–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. ↵
    1. Conners CK,
    2. Sitarenios G,
    3. Parker JD,
    4. et al
    .The revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity.J Abnorm Child Psychol 1998;26:257–68.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    1. Gillberg C,
    2. Gillberg IC
    .Infantile autism: a total population study of reduced optimality in the pre-, peri-, and neonatal period.J Autism Dev Disord 1983;13:153–66.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  36. ↵
    1. Brookes K,
    2. Xu X,
    3. Chen W,
    4. et al
    .The analysis of 51 genes in DSM-IV combined type attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: association signals in DRD4, DAT1 and 16 other genes.Mol Psychiatry 2006;11:934–53.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  37. ↵
    1. Team RCR
    (2012) A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).
  38. ↵
    1. Schaid DJ,
    2. Rowland CM,
    3. Tines DE,
    4. et al
    .Score tests for association between traits and haplotypes when linkage phase is ambiguous.Am J Hum Genet 2002;70:425–34.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  39. ↵
    1. Logan GD,
    2. Cowan WB,
    3. Davis KA
    .On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method.J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1984;10:276–91.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    1. Van Rooij D,
    2. Hoekstra PJ,
    3. Mennes M,
    4. et al
    .Distinguishing adolescents with ADHD from their unaffected siblings and healthy comparison subjects by neural activation patterns during response inhibition.Am J Psychiatry 2015;172:874–83.
    OpenUrl
  41. ↵
    1. Winkler AM,
    2. Webster MA,
    3. Vidaurre D,
    4. et al
    .Multi-level block permutation.Neuroimage 2015;123:253–68.
    OpenUrl
  42. ↵
    1. Smith SM,
    2. Fox PT,
    3. Miller KL,
    4. et al
    .Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009;106:13040–5.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    1. Hayes AF
    (2013) Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach (Guilford Press, New York (NY)).
  44. ↵
    1. Hans SL
    .Demographic and psychosocial characteristics of substance-abusing pregnant women.Clin Perinatol 1999;26:55–74.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  45. ↵
    1. Durston S,
    2. van Belle J,
    3. de Zeeuw P
    .Differentiating frontostriatal and fronto-cerebellar circuits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.Biol Psychiatry 2011;69:1178–84.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  46. ↵
    1. Nitsche MA,
    2. Lampe C,
    3. Antal A,
    4. et al
    .Dopaminergic modulation of long-lasting direct current-induced cortical excitability changes in the human motor cortex.Eur J Neurosci 2006;23:1651–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Noaín D,
    2. Avale ME,
    3. Wedemeyer C,
    4. et al
    .Identification of brain neurons expressing the dopamine D4 receptor gene using BAC transgenic mice.Eur J Neurosci 2006;24:2429–38.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Corbetta M,
    2. Shulman GL
    .Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain.Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3:201–15.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Belsky J,
    2. Pluess M
    .Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences.Psychol Bull 2009;135:885–908.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    1. Stuss DT,
    2. Murphy KJ,
    3. Binns MA,
    4. et al
    .Staying on the job: the frontal lobes control individual performance variability.Brain 2003;126:2363–80.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    1. Johnson MH,
    2. Jones EJ,
    3. Gliga T
    .Brain adaptation and alternative developmental trajectories.Dev Psychopathol 2015;27:425–42.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Durston S,
    2. Tottenham NT,
    3. Thomas KM,
    4. et al
    .Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children with and without ADHD.Biol Psychiatry 2003;53:871–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    1. Fassbender C,
    2. Schweitzer JB
    .Is there evidence for neural compensation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? A review of the functional neuroimaging literature.Clin Psychol Rev 2006;26:445–65.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Oak JN,
    2. Oldenhof J,
    3. Van Tol HHM
    .The dopamine D4 receptor: one decade of research.Eur J Pharmacol 2000;405:303–27.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  55. ↵
    1. Winstanley CA,
    2. Zeeb FD,
    3. Bedard A,
    4. et al
    .Dopaminergic modulation of the orbitofrontal cortex affects attention, motivation and impulsive responding in rats performing the five-choice serial reaction time task.Behav Brain Res 2010;210:263–72.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    1. Kodituwakku PW,
    2. May PA,
    3. Clericuzio CL,
    4. et al
    .Emotion-related learning in individuals prenatally exposed to alcohol: an investigation of the relation between set shifting, extinction of responses, and behavior.Neuropsychologia 2001;39:699–708.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  57. ↵
    1. Toplak ME,
    2. Jain U,
    3. Tannock R
    .Executive and motivational processes in adolescents with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).Behav Brain Funct 2005;1:8
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    1. Bryden DW,
    2. Roesch MR
    .Executive control signals in orbitofrontal cortex during response inhibition.J Neurosci 2015;35:3903–14.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  59. ↵
    1. Mulder MJ,
    2. Baeyens D,
    3. Davidson MC,
    4. et al
    .Familial vulnerability to ADHD affects activity in the cerebellum in addition to the prefrontal systems.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008;47:68–75.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  60. ↵
    1. Dempfle A,
    2. Scherag A,
    3. Hein R,
    4. et al
    .Gene-environment interactions for complex traits: definitions, methodological requirements and challenges.Eur J Hum Genet 2008;16:1164–72.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  61. ↵
    1. Shipton D,
    2. Tappin DM,
    3. Vadiveloo T,
    4. et al
    .Reliability of self reported smoking status by pregnant women for estimating smoking prevalence: a retrospective, cross sectional study.BMJ 2009;339:b4347
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  62. ↵
    1. Obel C,
    2. Zhu JL,
    3. Olsen J,
    4. et al
    .The risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children exposed to maternal smoking during pregnancy — a re-examination using a sibling design.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2016;57:532–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  63. ↵
    1. David SP,
    2. Munafo MR,
    3. Murphy MFG,
    4. et al
    .Genetic variation in the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene and smoking cessation: follow-up of a randomised clinical trial of transdermal nicotine patch.Pharmacogenomics J 2008;8:122–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    1. Malanga CJ,
    2. Kosofsky BE
    .Does drug abuse beget drug abuse? Behavioral analysis of addiction liability in animal models of prenatal drug exposure.Brain Res Dev Brain Res 2003;147:47–57.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    1. Thapar A,
    2. Rice F,
    3. Hay D,
    4. et al
    .Prenatal smoking might not cause attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence from a novel design.Biol Psychiatry 2009;66:722–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience: 42 (2)
J Psychiatry Neurosci
Vol. 42, Issue 2
1 Mar 2017
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author

Article tools

Respond to this article
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
To sign up for email alerts or to access your current email alerts, enter your email address below:
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on JPN.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Effects of dopaminergic genes, prenatal adversities, and their interaction on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and neural correlates of response inhibition
(Your Name) has sent you a message from JPN
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the JPN web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Effects of dopaminergic genes, prenatal adversities, and their interaction on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and neural correlates of response inhibition
Dennis van der Meer, Catharina A. Hartman, Daan van Rooij, Barbara Franke, Dirk J. Heslenfeld, Jaap Oosterlaan, Stephen V. Faraone, Jan K. Buitelaar, Pieter J. Hoekstra
J Psychiatry Neurosci Mar 2017, 42 (2) 113-121; DOI: 10.1503/jpn.150350

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
‍ Request Permissions
Share
Effects of dopaminergic genes, prenatal adversities, and their interaction on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and neural correlates of response inhibition
Dennis van der Meer, Catharina A. Hartman, Daan van Rooij, Barbara Franke, Dirk J. Heslenfeld, Jaap Oosterlaan, Stephen V. Faraone, Jan K. Buitelaar, Pieter J. Hoekstra
J Psychiatry Neurosci Mar 2017, 42 (2) 113-121; DOI: 10.1503/jpn.150350
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like

Related Articles

  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • No citing articles found.
  • Google Scholar

Similar Articles

Content

  • Current issue
  • Past issues
  • Collections
  • Alerts
  • RSS

Authors & Reviewers

  • Overview for Authors
  • Submit a manuscript
  • Manuscript Submission Checklist

About

  • General Information
  • Staff
  • Editorial Board
  • Contact Us
  • Advertising
  • Reprints
  • Copyright and Permissions
  • Accessibility
  • CMA Civility Standards
CMAJ Group

Copyright 2023, CMA Impact Inc. or its licensors. All rights reserved. ISSN 1180-4882.

All editorial matter in JPN represents the opinions of the authors and not necessarily those of the Canadian Medical Association or its subsidiaries.
To receive any of these resources in an accessible format, please contact us at CMAJ Group, 500-1410 Blair Towers Place, Ottawa ON, K1J 9B9; p: 1-888-855-2555; e: [email protected].
View CMA's Accessibility policy.

Powered by HighWire